#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Distinguishing mild cognitive impairment from healthy aging and Alzheimer’s Disease: The contribution of the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS)


Authors: Helena S. Moreira aff001;  Ana Sofia Costa aff002;  Álvaro Machado aff002;  São Luís Castro aff001;  César F. Lima aff001;  Selene G. Vicente aff001
Authors place of work: Centre for Psychology at University of Porto, Porto, Portugal aff001;  Neurocognition Unit, Department of Neurology, Hospital de Braga, Braga, Portugal aff002;  Department of Neurology, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany aff003;  JARA Institute Molecular Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, Aachen, Germany aff004;  Instituto Universitário de Lisboa (ISCTE-IUL), Lisboa, Portugal aff005
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 14(9)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221873

Summary

Executive functions are affected differently in healthy aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD), and evaluating them is important for differential diagnosis. The INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) is a brief neuropsychological screening tool, developed to assess executive dysfunction in neurodegenerative disorders. Goals: We aimed to examine whether and how MCI patients can be differentiated from cognitively healthy controls (HC) and mild to moderate AD patients based on IFS performance. We also explored how IFS scores are associated with age, years of education, and depressive/anxious symptoms (as assessed by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale). Method: IFS total scores were compared between 26 HC, 32 MCI and 21 mild to moderate AD patients. The three groups were matched for age and education. The Area Under the Curve (AUC) was analyzed and optimal cut-offs were determined. Results: Healthy participants had higher IFS scores than both clinical groups, and MCI patients had higher scores than AD patients. IFS showed high diagnostic accuracy for the detection of MCI (AUC = .89, p < .001) and AD (AUC = .99, p < .001), and for the differentiation between the clinical groups (AUC = .76, p < .001). We provide optimal cut-offs for the identification of MCI and AD and for their differentiation. We also found that, in general, higher education predicted higher IFS scores (no associations with age and depressive/anxious symptoms were observed). Altogether, these findings indicate that evaluating executive functions with the IFS can be valuable for the identification of MCI, a high-risk group for dementia, and for differentiating this condition from healthy aging and AD.

Keywords:

Biology and life sciences – Neuroscience – Cognitive science – Learning and memory – Psychology – Social sciences – Medicine and health sciences – Diagnostic medicine – Neurology – Cognitive neurology – Cognitive impairment – Cognitive neuroscience – Mental health and psychiatry – dementia – Alzheimer's disease – Neurodegenerative diseases – Cognition – memory – Mood disorders – depression – Neuropsychological testing – Working memory – Neuropsychology

Introduction

Executive function (EF) is an overarching term referring to the coordinated operation of specific cognitive processes (e.g., planning, working memory, self-monitoring) that direct cognition, emotion and motor activity during the accomplishment of goals, allowing individuals to respond adaptively to their environment [1]. Structural and functional neuroimaging studies [2,3] indicate that the prefrontal cortex is pivotal for EF, though other cortical (e.g., parietal areas [4]) and subcortical (e.g., cerebellum [5]) structures are also involved.

EF seems to follow an inverted U trajectory across the lifespan, declining in late adult years [6,7]. Cross-sectional [8] and longitudinal [9] studies show that EF are the first cognitive functions to decline with aging, and the magnitude of this decline is greater than the one observed for cognitive domains such as episodic memory, reasoning, and spatial abilities [7]. Working memory [10], inhibition, and planning [11,12] are the most affected EF processes, accounting for variance in cognitive domains such as learning and episodic memory [13,14]. This age-related decline in EF has been associated with structural and functional changes in frontal lobe areas [9,15,16]. Importantly, such decline is generally subtle and does not compromise autonomy. When it does, there is a risk of association with neurodegenerative disorders.

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) represents a stage of cognitive function between the expected decline seen in healthy aging and that seen in dementia, i.e., individuals with MCI have a more pronounced cognitive impairment than what would be expected for their age and education, but do not meet functional criteria for dementia [17,18]. Longitudinal studies indicate higher rates of progression to dementia in MCI patients vs. healthy controls [19,20], a finding highlighting the predictive value of this clinical entity. There is considerable heterogeneity regarding the cause and prognosis of MCI. Most often, patients develop Alzheimer’s dementia (AD) [21]. However, MCI can also result from other degenerative conditions such as Parkinson’s disease [22] and frontotemporal lobar degeneration [23], or non-degenerative disorders such as cerebrovascular pathologies [24], brain tumors and medication [21]. Based on clinical presentation, MCI has been classified into four subtypes: amnestic MCI single domain, amnestic MCI multiple-domain, non-amnestic MCI single domain, and non-amnestic MCI multiple-domain [18,25]. Deficits in EF are common in amnestic multiple domain and non-amnestic single or multiple-domain MCI, with patients performing worse than HC in EF tasks [10,26]. These impairments affect patients’ functionality [27] and are associated with depressive and anxious symptoms [28]. Furthermore, MCI patients who present with executive and memory deficits combined are at a particularly high risk of progression to dementia (as compared to single domain amnestic or dysexecutive MCI) [29]. However, the study of EF in MCI remains a relatively poorly explored topic, particularly in what respects to comparisons with AD patients. Such studies are important for a better understanding of executive impairments in different conditions, and they can also contribute to better differential diagnosis.

AD is typically characterized by impairments in episodic memory [30,31], but discernable deficits in EF are also often present in components such as verbal inhibition [32,33], planning [12], abstraction [30] and working memory [34]. Executive dysfunction in AD patients is associated with increased frontoparietal brain activity in relation to task demands in functional MRI and positron emission tomography studies [35]. Deficits in EF impact on AD patients’ daily life, being associated with functional disability, with a greater need for care [36], and with the emergence of neuropsychiatric symptoms such as agitation, disinhibition [37], and psychotic symptoms [38].

The INECO Frontal Screening (IFS) [39] is a brief screening tool that assesses three executive domains (response inhibition and set shifting, abstraction, and working memory) and presents good psychometric properties (for a review, see Moreira and colleagues [40]). The IFS can discriminate controls from patients with frontotemporal dementia (FTD) [39,41,42] and AD [33,39,42], and it can also discriminate between some clinical groups, with FTD patients scoring lower than those with major depression [41] and AD [39,43]. To our knowledge, though, the utility of this tool in the context of MCI remains unknown. Previous studies indicate that the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [44], a screening tool for global cognition, can distinguish MCI from HC and early stage AD patients [45,46]. Can EF, as assessed with the IFS, also distinguish MCI from the other groups? Another relevant issue concerns the potential effects of age and education on IFS performance, effects that might have implications for the use of this tool in MCI and in clinical practice more broadly. A number of studies report positive effects of education [33,47,48], but as for age results are mixed: some studies report negative effects of age on IFS performance [33], while others do not [47,48].

The goal of the current study was to investigate whether MCI patients can be differentiated from healthy controls and AD patients based on EF abilities, using IFS. The contribution of each IFS subtest was also analyzed, and optimal cut-off points to detect MCI and AD, as well as to differentiate these two clinical groups, were established. We hypothesized that MCI patients would present lower performance than HC on EF, as indexed by lower IFS scores, but higher performance than AD patients. Additionally, the influence of age, years of education and depressive/anxious symptomatology (as indexed by the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale; HADS) [49] on IFS performance was examined. We expected IFS performance to be negatively influenced by age and depressive/anxious symptoms and positively influenced by years of education.

Materials and methods

Participants

A total of 79 participants were included in this study: 26 HC, 32 MCI patients and 21 AD patients. Patients were retrospectively selected from a specialized memory outpatient clinic, and had their baseline neurological and neuropsychological assessment between 2013 and 2015 (all the results reported here are based on this initial baseline assessment). The Ethics Committee for Health of Hospital de Braga (Comissão de Ética para a Saúde Hospital de Braga) approved the retrospective analysis of the patients’ clinical information (CESHB 054/2016) and the study was conducted in agreement with the Helsinki Declaration. All data were anonymized before being accessed for research purposes. We were not required by the ethics committee to obtain informed consent from the patients, as the clinical data were entirely extracted from the available clinical records, i.e., no additional tests were specifically administered for the current study. This is in agreement with the Portuguese law N°. 12/2005 of 26 January. Written informed consent was obtained from all HC participants.

Patients were consecutively included in the study if they had (1) criteria for MCI or AD, and (2) available information regarding the MMSE, MoCA, HADS and IFS total and subtest scores. They were classified as MCI using Petersen and colleagues [21] revised criteria: (1) objective impairment in formal neuropsychological measures (total score on MMSE and MoCA at least 1.5 SD below the demographically corrected mean) [50,45] and (2) preserved activities of daily living. According to the standardized neuropsychological assessment, 19 MCI patients were classified as “amnestic MCI” (multiple-domain) and 13 as “non-amnestic MCI” (single or multiple-domain). For each patient, the probable etiology of MCI was determined by an experienced neurologist, based on detailed diagnostic work-up (including clinical history and neurological, neuropsychological, and functional and/or structural neuroimaging examinations). This information pointed to potential neurodegenerative pathology in most cases. Cases with clinical records of primary psychiatric disease, significant vascular pathology and other focal lesions (e.g., traumatic brain injury) were excluded. Diagnosis of AD was based on standard diagnostic procedures, according to national and international diagnostic guidelines for probable AD [51], and included the clinical history, neurological examination, routine blood tests, clinical imaging of the brain (computed tomography or magnetic resonance), and a comprehensive neuropsychological assessment (for protocol details see Costa et al. [52]). In the present study, the IFS was not considered for diagnostic purposes. For some patients, we also had information on cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers of AD (AΒ, total tau, and phospho-tau, AΒ/tau ratio) and/or functional imaging (fludeoxyglucose F18 positron emission tomography or single-photon emission computed tomography). Most patients were over 65-years-old when diagnosed, and therefore potentially reflect sporadic rather than early-onset familial cases of AD [53]; only four of them were younger, but none of these had familial history of the disease and/or mutations in genes APOɛ4, PSEN1, PSEN 2 and APP. Only patients with mild-to-moderate dementia severity were included (stages 4 and 5 of Global Deterioration Scale) [54], all of them with MMSE scores above 10 (mild and moderate AD stage) [55].

The presence of depressive symptoms was not an exclusion criterion, given its prodromal value in MCI [56,57] and links with neuropathologic processes of AD [58]. However, in general, the magnitude of these symptoms was similar across groups (p = .79, see Table 1).

Tab. 1.

Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of Healthy Controls (HC), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) participants.

<h2>Demographic and neuropsychological characteristics of Healthy Controls (HC), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) participants.</h2>

HC participants were volunteers recruited in the community and they were matched with the MCI and AD patients for age and education. All of them were autonomous and had no known history of alcoholism/substance abuse, brain injury, neurological/psychiatric conditions, or other significant medical conditions that could affect cognition. For inclusion, normal performance on MMSE and MoCA, as indicated by scores above the age and education corrected cut-offs, was required [50, 45].

Neuropsychological assessment

The sociodemographic and clinical data were collected through a clinical interview or obtained from patient records. The MMSE and MoCA were used as measures of global cognition. Symptoms of depression and anxiety were assessed with the HADS. All participants completed the Portuguese version of IFS [33]. The IFS takes around 10 minutes to administer and comprises eight subtests that cover three domains (response inhibition and set shifting, abstraction, and working memory), and inspect the following specific processes: motor programming (Luria’s motor series fist, edge, palm), sensitivity to interference (conflicting instructions), inhibitory control (Go/no Go), verbal inhibitory control (modified Hayling test), abstraction capacity (proverb interpretation), working memory for digits (backward digit span), verbal working memory (months of the year backward), and spatial working memory (modified Corsi block tapping test). The tasks motor series, conflicting instructions, and Go/no Go were taken from Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) [59], a widely used screening test of EF. The other ones are specific to the IFS and were selected to optimize the sensitivity of this tool in comparison to the FAB. More comparative research is needed, but the FAB was found to discriminate between controls and AD patients less well than IFS (for a review see Moreira et al., [40]). IFS total score ranges between 0 and 30, with lower scores suggesting worse executive functioning.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were used for sample characterization. A multiple linear regression (enter method) was performed to examine the extent to which sociodemographic variables (education and age) and affective symptoms (anxious/depressive) influence IFS total score, even when the cognitive impairment associated to each diagnostic group is accounted for (i.e., Group was also included in the model). Comparisons between the three groups in IFS total and subtests’ scores were explored with one-way ANOVAs, and follow-up paired comparisons were Bonferroni corrected. Values of skewness and kurtosis were always below 2 and 7, respectively (skewness, range = -1.38–1.30; kurtosis, range = -1.19–1.14), suggesting that there is no large departure from normality in the data [60]. The diagnostic accuracy of the IFS total score for the detection of MCI and AD patients was assessed with receiver operation characteristics (ROC) curve analyses. The optimal cut-offs scores were selected based on the Youden index, with higher values indicating best sensitivity (the proportion of participants with cognitive impairment correctly identified as such) and specificity (the proportion of participants without cognitive impairment correctly identified as such). Finally, a discriminant analysis was carried out to determine the contribution of each IFS subtest for the group classification. All effects were considered significant at p < .05. Analysis were done using IBM SPSS® software (version 24).

Results

Sample characterization and influence of age, education, depressive/anxious symptoms on IFS scores

The characteristics of HC participants, MCI and AD patients are summarized in Table 1. The three groups did not differ in age [F (2,78) = 2.12, p = .13, ηp2 = .05], years of education [F (2,78) = 0.85, p = .43, ηp2 = .02), sex [χ2 (2) = 1.7, p = .43] and in HADS total score [F (2,61) = 0.23, p = .79, ηp2 = .01]. Note that the education level of our sample is lower than the observed in other similar studies [39,42,43], but it is representative of the majority of Portuguese elderly population. According to Censos 2011 [61], the majority of people over 65 years in Portugal completed only 4 years of formal education, corresponding to 1st Cycle of Basic Education (67.27%, 62.65%, and 56.80% of the people aged 65–69 years, 70–74 years, and 75–79 years, respectively).

MMSE scores significantly differed between the three groups, as expected [F (2,78) = 133.38, p < .001, ηp2 = .78]; they were lower in the AD group than in both MCI (p < .001) and HC (p < .001) groups, and MCI patients also underperformed HC participants (p = .001). Please note that for MMSE there was a very low range in HC (between 27–30), reinforcing the well documented celling effect of this measure [62]. Significant differences between groups were also found for MoCA total scores [F (2,75) = 76.84, p < .001, ηp2 = .68]: the AD group presented the lowest scores, followed by MCI and HC groups, that also significantly differed from each other (all ps < .001).

A multiple regression analysis was conducted with group, age, years of education and HADS total score as independent variables and IFS total score as dependent variable. The resulting model explained 73% of the IFS variance [adjusted R2 = 0.73, F (4,61) = 41.13, p < .001]. Group was the strongest predictor (ß = 0.76, t = 11.19, p < .001), but education also significantly predicted unique variance in IFS performance (ß = 0.37, t = 5.22, p < .001), with more years of education being associated with better results in this tool. Neither age nor HADS total score were significant predictors (p = .67 and p = .83, respectively).

Sensitivity of the IFS to differentiate MCI patients from HC and AD patients

The performance of HC participants, MCI and AD patients on the IFS is depicted in Fig 1. As hypothesized, significant differences were found between the three groups [F (2, 78) = 47.57, p < .001, ηp2 = 0.56]. Specifically, MCI patients (M = 15.47; SD = 5.63; range = 4–27) had lower total scores than HC (M = 23.44; SD = 2.86; range = 16.5–29; p < .001) but higher than AD patients (M = 9.74; SD = 5.52; range = 3–20; p < .001). In order to determine if these differences remain significant after controlling for variables that we have shown to predict IFS performance (years of education), we conducted an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA). The group effect was confirmed even when this confounding variable was controlled [F (2,75) = 14. 58, p < .001, ηp2 = .67], between all of the three groups: MCI patients vs. HC participants (p < .001), MCI patients vs. AD patients (p < .001) and AD vs. HC participants (p < .001). The same results were obtained when the ANCOVA also included sex as a covariate.

Fig. 1.

Total IFS total scores for control and clinical groups.

<h2>Total IFS total scores for control and clinical groups.</h2>

HC, Healthy Controls; MCI, patients with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI); AD, patients with Alzheimer’s dementia. Error bars represent standard error. * p < .05.

Since there were two identifiable subtypes within our sample of MCI participants (amnestic and non-amnestic), we also conducted an additional exploratory analysis to check if there were discernible differences between them in IFS total and subtests scores. No differences were found in IFS total score (p = .23) or subscores (ps > .74), apart from a marginally significant effect in the Conflicting Instructions subtest [with amnestic MCI performing slightly higher than non-amnestic, t(17.62) = 2.10, p = .05].

ROC curve analyses were carried out to evaluate the diagnostic accuracy of the IFS (see Fig 2). Regarding the discrimination between HC and MCI participants, this analysis generated an optimal cut-off of 20 points, with 92% sensitivity and 81% specificity (AUC of 0.89, CI = 0.80–0. 98). In the discrimination between HC and AD participants, the optimal cut-off was 15 points, with 100% sensitivity of and 81% specificity (AUC of 0.99, CI = 0.96–1.00). A cut-off of 12.5 points reached the best sensitivity (78%) and specificity (76%) in the differentiation between clinical groups of MCI and AD patients (AUC of 0.76, CI = 0.62–0.90).

Fig. 2.

Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves of the IFS in the differentiation between control and clinical groups, as well as between the two clinical groups.

<h2>Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curves of the IFS in the differentiation between control and clinical groups, as well as between the two clinical groups.</h2>

HC from MCI patients (left) and AD patients (center); MCI from AD patients (right).

To examine the performance of the three groups in specific executive domains, the scores of each IFS subtest were compared (see Table 2). The effect of group was largest for the Hayling test [F (2,76) = 44.58, p < .001, η p2 = .54], Proverbs interpretation [F (2,76) = 31.89, p < .001, ηp2 = .46], Backward digit span [F (2,76) = 23.94, p < .001, ηp2 = .39], and Motor series [F (2,76) = 19.74, ηp2 = .34], suggesting that these were the subtests that were more sensitive to general variation across groups. For the Hayling test, Proverbs interpretation, and Backward digit span, AD and MCI patients scored lower than HC, but no significant differences were found between AD and MCI patients (Hayling test, HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p < .001 and MCI vs. AD, p = .66; Proverbs interpretation, HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p < .001 and MCI vs. AD, p = .26; Backward digit span, HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p < .001 and MCI vs. AD, p = .79). For the Motor series, significant differences were obtained between the three groups, with AD patients presenting the worst results, followed by MCI patients and by the HC (HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p < .001 and MCI vs. AD, p = .05). The remaining four subtests (Conflicting instructions, Go/no go, Verbal working memory and Spatial working memory) were not sensitive to differences between HC and MCI patients, but they discriminated between HC and AD patients, and between MCI and AD patients (Conflicting instructions: F (2,76) = 11.41, p < .001, ηp2 = .21; HC vs. AD, p <. 001; HC vs. MCI, p = .81 and MCI vs. AD, p = .001; Go /no go: F (2,76) = 9.03, p < .001; ηp2 = .19; HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p = 1.00 and MCI vs. AD, p = .003; Verbal working memory: F (2,76) = 16.32, p < .001, ηp2 = .30; HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p = .21 and MCI vs. AD, p < .001; Spatial working memory: F (2,76) = 12.19, p < .001, ηp2 = .24; HC vs. AD, p < .001; HC vs. MCI, p = .06 and MCI vs. AD, p = .02).

Tab. 2.

Scores in IFS subtests of Healthy Controls (HC), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) patients and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) patients.

<h2>Scores in IFS subtests of Healthy Controls (HC), Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI) patients and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) patients.</h2>

Finally, a discriminant analysis (enter method) was conducted to further understand how IFS scores could be informative about differences between groups. The discriminant function was able to correctly classify the group membership of 79% of the patients (Wilks' Lambda = 0.25; χ2 (16) = 98.77, p < .001). The three subtests that presented the highest contribution were: 81% in the Hayling test, 69% in the Proverb interpretation, and 63% in the Backward digit span. Note that these were the same subtests that showed larger effect sizes in the previously presented comparisons across groups.

Discussion

EF are higher-order processes that are differentially compromised in healthy aging, MCI and AD. In previous studies, the IFS [39] has shown good discriminant accuracy between healthy participants and patients with dementia, and also between patients with different types of dementia. Nonetheless, its utility in MCI–a stage of cognitive decline that might progress to dementia—had not yet been explored. The present study provides four main new findings. First, MCI patients can be distinguished from healthy participants and early to moderate AD patients based on the IFS total score. Specifically, the MCI group had lower scores than controls and higher scores than patients with AD. Second, IFS performance was not influenced by age or depressive/anxious symptoms but was positively influenced by years of education. Nonetheless, its discriminative power in MCI vs. HC, MCI vs. AD and MCI vs. AD remains significant even when controlling for education. Third, cut-off scores of 20 and 12.5 provided optimal sensitivity and specificity in the identification of MCI vs. controls and of MCI vs. AD, respectively. Fourth, performance on IFS subtests can also be informative to differentiate between healthy elderlies, MCI and AD patients. These conclusions are discussed in detail below.

The IFS total score accurately distinguished MCI patients from HC and AD patients. Finding that the IFS differentiates AD from HC adds to a number of previous studies, and highlights again the utility of this screening tool in the context of this condition [33,39,42,43]. However, the current study is the first to provide evidence for the utility of IFS in the context of MCI: such utility is not only seen in analyses of score differences between groups, but also in subtests scores, discriminant analysis and analyses of diagnostic accuracy (AUC = .89 for MCI vs. HC, and AUC = .76 for MCI vs. AD). The existence of executive impairments in MCI has been documented before [26,63], but evidence for the utility of brief screening tools to assess them is limited. This finding is valuable both for research and clinical purposes, for instance for the neuropsychological characterization and newly diagnosed MCI patients (and monitoring of progression), but also for differential diagnosis (between MCI and AD). Nonetheless, we should note that AUC values were higher between MCI and HC than between MCI and AD, suggesting that the IFS might be particularly useful for the early detection of impairments in people at risk of developing dementia (and relatively less for the differentiation between MCI and AD patients). We found that a IFS cut-off of 20 reaches high values of sensitivity (.92) and specificity (.81) in the discrimination between MCI and HC. Such sensitivity and specificity values are comparable to those presented by MoCA [45], a well-established tool specifically developed for this purpose.

An analysis of subtests scores showed that these can also be informative. Subtests of verbal inhibitory control (Hayling subtest), abstraction (Proverbs), and verbal working memory (Backward digit span) were the most informative for differences between HC and MCI. However, they were less sensitive to the differences between MCI and AD, i.e., between stages of subtle cognitive impairment and dementia. The Motor series subtest, in turn, was able to discriminate between the three groups. Tasks which explore sensitivity to interference (Conflicting instructions), inhibitory control (Go no-go) and working memory (verbal and spatial working memory subtests) were more valuable for the differentiation between MCI and AD, presenting relatively lower potential for the identification of subtle deficits of MCI in comparison with HC. Thus, albeit the information provided by the IFS does not replace medical, comprehensive neuropsychological and neuroimaging techniques, the examination of total and subtests scores produces relevant information that can significantly contribute to the differential diagnosis between HC, MCI and AD patients. Which subtest(s) might be more informative will depend on the question being asked (e.g., discrimination between HC and MCI, or discrimination between HC and AD).

Contrary to our hypothesis, age did not predict the performance in IFS. This is not in line with the previous study of Moreira and colleagues [33], with a Portuguese normative sample, but is consistent with the recent study of Sanjurjo et al. [48] that aimed to evaluate the impact of demographic variables on IFS performance of healthy participants. Nonsignificant correlations between age and IFS total score were also found in the study of Ihnen and colleagues [47] with a dementia group. The absence of an age effect in IFS has been previously argued to reflect a celling effect of the measure [48], sometimes observed in the context of short screening tools [64]. Nonetheless, studies with other classic executive measures such as task-switching [8,65], verbal fluency, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test-Modified [66] also reported limited age effects. We should note, however, that the age range in the current study is somewhat limited, for instance in comparison to the age range of a previous study by our group in which age effects on the IFS were found [33]. Moreover, our sample is relatively small and all participants were included in the multiple regression analysis regardless of their diagnostic group (instead of including only the control ones)–this was done to increase statistical power, but it might have slightly biased the results. Thus, this question remains open and calls for more systematic research in the future. Intra-individual longitudinal analysis over time, for example, would be a more informative and stronger test to the influence of age on IFS performance. Education, in turn, significantly accounts for IFS performance, with more formal years of education being associated with higher scores. This effect has been consistently reported in previous studies with the IFS [33,47,48] but also with other executive screening measures such as the FAB [67]. Therefore, our study underlines the importance of considering education in the interpretation of the performance in executive tools [68]. Higher education levels seem to increase the contact with the evaluation contexts and to contribute to cognitive reserve, exerting a protective effect on the decline associated with healthy aging in EF [69]. Importantly, despite the influence of education, the discriminative power of this tool in the detection of EF deficits in MCI and in their differentiation from those of AD patients remains after accounting for this variable.

In an exploratory analysis, we compared the IFS total scores of patients with different MCI subtypes and found that they did not differ significantly. This is consistent with previous studies that suggest that both amnestic multiple-domain and non-amnestic single and multiple-domain MCI subtypes presented executive deficits [26,29]. However, our groups were relatively small (n = 19 for amnestic MCI and n = 13 for non-amnestic MCI) and more studies with bigger samples will be essential to provide more reliable conclusions. Another issue that deserves more attention in future studies is the relationship between the IFS and measures of functional ability. It was established that performance in instrumental activities of daily living is highly related with EF [70]. Hence, it would be expected that performance in IFS could predict the scores on functionality measures. This information is even more relevant since the differential diagnosis between MCI and dementia relies primarily in the decline in functionality [18,70].

Some limitations of our study should be acknowledged. In addition to the relatively small sample size, we did not have access to patients’ longitudinal data. Such information would be valuable particularly for MCI patients, not only to better clarify the aetiology of their cognitive impairment, but also to examine how the impairment changes over time and the rates of progression to dementia. In future work it will be important to determine the extent to which brief tools such as the IFS can provide useful information to predict cognitive and functional outcomes of MCI patients over time.

Conclusion

This study produced evidence regarding the utility of the IFS for the identification of MCI, a well-established condition associated with a higher risk of progression to dementia. The early detection of this condition through a brief screening test enables prompting implementation of intervention strategies and managing of risk factors, thus holding potential to prevent a faster progression. Likewise, we found that performance in IFS can discriminate not only between normal aging and MCI and AD but also between these two clinical groups. The contribution of this tool for the differential diagnosis of these groups might be helpful in clinic settings where they are increasingly prevalent. Finally, our results underline the need of normative data from different levels of education, in order to avoid biased interpretations.

Supporting information

S1 Dataset [pdf]
Minimal anonymized dataset.


Zdroje

1. Goldstein S, Naglieri JA, Princiotta D, Otero TM. Introduction: A History of Executive Functioning as a Theoretical and Clinical Construct. In: Goldstein S, Naglieri JÁ, editors. Handbook of Executive Functioning. New York: Springer; 2013. pp.24–44.

2. Yuan P, Raz N. Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: A meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014 May 42; 180–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005 24568942

3. Robinson H, Calamia M, Gläscher J, Bruss J, Tranel D. Neuroanatomical Correlates of Executive Functions: A Neuropsychological Approach Using the EXAMINER Battery. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013 Jun 13;20(01):52–63. doi: 10.1017/S135561771300060X 23759126

4. Burzynska AZ, Nagel IE, Preuschhof C, Gluth S, Bäckman L, Li S-C, et al. Cortical thickness is linked to executive functioning in adulthood and aging. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011 Jul 7;33(7):1607–1620. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21311 21739526

5. Marvel CL, Desmond JE. Functional Topography of the Cerebellum in Verbal Working Memory. Neuropsychology Rev. 2010 Jun 22;20(3):271–9. doi: 10.1007/s11065-010-9137-7 20563894

6. Hirsiger S, Koppelmans V, Mérillat S, Erdin C, Narkhede A, Brickman AM, et al. Executive Functions in Healthy Older Adults Are Differentially Related to Macro- and Microstructural White Matter Characteristics of the Cerebral Lobes. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017 Nov 30;9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00373 29249957

7. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Festini SB, Jantz TK. Executive Functions and Neurocognitive Aging. In: Schaie KW, Willis SL, editors. Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. Oxford: Elsevier; 2016. pp. 245–262.

8. Aging Verhaeghen P. and Executive Control: Reports of a Demise Greatly Exaggerated. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2011 May 24;20(3):174–80. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408772

9. Fjell AM, Sneve MH, Grydeland H, Storsve AB, Walhovd KB. The Disconnected Brain and Executive Function Decline in Aging. Cereb. Cortex. 2016 Apr 12. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhw082 27073220

10. Kirova A-M, Bays RB, Lagalwar S. Working Memory and Executive Function Decline across Normal Aging, Mild Cognitive Impairment, and Alzheimer’s Disease. Biomed Res Int. 2015;2015:1–9. doi: 10.1155/2015/748212 26550575

11. Amieva H, Phillips L, Della Sala S. Behavioral dysexecutive symptoms in normal aging. Brain Cogn. 2003 Nov;53(2):129–32. doi: 10.1016/s0278-2626(03)00094-0 14607132

12. Espinosa A, Alegret M, Boada M, Vinyes SG, Valero S, Martinéz-Lage P, et al. Ecological assessment of executive functions in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer’s disease. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2009 Jul 2;15(05):751. doi: 10.1017/s135561770999035x 19570310

13. Angel L, Bastin C, Genon S, Salmon E, Fay S, Balteau E, et al. Neural correlates of successful memory retrieval in aging: Do executive functioning and task difficulty matter? Brain Res. 2016 Jan;1631:53–71. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.10.009 26541580

14. Princiotta D, DeVries M, Goldstein S. Executive Functioning as a Mediator of Age-Related Cognitive Decline in Adults. In: Goldstein S, Naglieri JÁ, editors. Handbook of Executive Functioning. New York: Springer; 2013.pp. 143–155

15. Fjell AM, Walhovd KB. Structural Brain Changes in Aging: Courses, Causes and Cognitive Consequences. Rev Neurosci. 2010 Jan;21(3). doi: 10.1515/revneuro.2010.21.3.187

16. Fernandez-Ruiz J, Peltsch A, Alahyane N, Brien DC, Coe BC, Garcia A, et al. Age related prefrontal compensatory mechanisms for inhibitory control in the antisaccade task. Neuroimage. 2018 Jan;165:92–101. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.10.001 28988829

17. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild Cognitive Impairment. Arch Neurol. 1999 Mar 1;56(3):303. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303 10190820

18. Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Boeve BF, Geda YE, Ivnik RJ, et al. Mild Cognitive Impairment: ten years later. Arch Neurol. 2009 Dec 1;66(12). doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.266 20008648

19. Bermejo-Pareja F, Contador I, Trincado R, Lora D, Sánchez-Ferro Á, Mitchell AJ, et al. Prognostic Significance of Mild Cognitive Impairment Subtypes for Dementia and Mortality: Data from the NEDICES Cohort. J Alzheimers Dis. 2016 Feb 2;50(3):719–31. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150625 26757038

20. Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Mielke MM, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, Christianson TJH, et al. Higher risk of progression to dementia in mild cognitive impairment cases who revert to normal. Neurology. 2013 Dec 18;82(4):317–325. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000055 24353333

21. Petersen RC, Caracciolo B, Brayne C, Gauthier S, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept in evolution. J Intern Med. 2014 Mar;275(3):214–28. doi: 10.1111/joim.12190 24605806

22. Monastero R, Cicero CE, Baschi R, Davì M, Luca A., Restivo V, et al. … Mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease: the Parkinson's disease cognitive study (PACOS). J Neurol.2018 May; 265 (5):1050–1058. doi: 10.1007/s00415-018-8800-4 29478221

23. Domoto-Reilly K, Sapolsky D, Negreira A, Brickhouse M, Dickerson B. Mild cognitive impairment of frontotemporal lobar degeneration subtypes: Clinical and imaging characteristics. Alzheimers Dement. 2012 Jul;8(4):P624. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.05.1662

24. Consoli A, Pasi M, Pantoni L. Vascular mild cognitive impairment: concept, definition, and directions for future studies. Aging Clin Exp Res. 2012 Apr;24(2):113–116. doi: 10.1007/bf03325158 22842831

25. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, et al. Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. 2004 Aug 24; 256: 240–246. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x 15324367

26. Brandt J, Aretouli E, Neijstrom E, Samek J, Manning K, Albert MS, et al. Selectivity of executive function deficits in mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(5):607–618. doi: 10.1037/a0015851 19702414

27. Putcha D, Tremont G. Predictors of independence in instrumental activities of daily living: Amnestic versus nonamnestic MCI. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2016 May 31;38(9):991–1004. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1181716 27240585

28. Rosenberg PB, Mielke MM, Appleby B, Oh E, Leoutsakos J-M, Lyketsos CG. Neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI subtypes: the importance of executive dysfunction. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2010 Sep 15;26(4):364–372. doi: 10.1002/gps.2535 20845402

29. Hessen E, Reinvang I, Eliassen CF, Nordlund A, Gjerstad L, Fladby T, et al. The Combination of Dysexecutive and Amnestic Deficits Strongly Predicts Conversion to Dementia in Young Mild Cognitive Impairment Patients: A Report from the Gothenburg-Oslo MCI Study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2014 Apr 9;4(1):76–85. doi: 10.1159/000360282 24847346

30. Baudic S, Barba GD, Thibaudet MC, Smagghe A, Rem P, Traykov L. Executive function deficits in early Alzheimer’s disease and their relations with episodic memory. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2006 Jan; 21: 15–21. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.002 16125364

31. Bejanin A, Schonhaut DR, La Joie R, Kramer JH, Baker SL, Sosa N. et al. Tau pathology and neurodegeneration contribute to cognitive impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2017 Oct 7;140: 3286–3300. doi: 10.1093/brain/awx243 29053874

32. Amieva H, Phillips LH, Della Salla S, Henry JD. Inhibitory functioning in Alzheimer Disease. Brain. 2004 May 1;127 (5): 949–964. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh045 14645147

33. Moreira H, Lima C, Vicente S. Examining Executive Dysfunction with the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) Frontal Screening (IFS). Normative Values from a Healthy Sample and Clinical Utility in Alzheimer’s Disease. J Alzheimers Dis. 2014 March 25;42(1): 261–273. doi: 10.3233/JAD-132348 24840570

34. Belleville S, Chertkow H, Gauthier S. Working memory and control of attention in persons with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychology. 2007 jul; 21(4): 458–469. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.458 17605579

35. Firbank M, Kobeleva X, Cherry G, Killen A, Gallagher P, Burn DJ, et al. Neural correlates of attention‐executive dysfunction in lewy body dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016 Mar;37(3):1254–70. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23100 26705763

36. Stokholm J, Vogel A, Gade A, Waldemar G. Heterogeneity in Executive Impairment in Patients with Very Mild Alzheimer’s Disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2006;22(1):54–9. doi: 10.1159/000093262 16682794

37. Chen ST, Sultzer DL, Hinkin CH. Executive dysfunction in Alzheimer’s: association with neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional impairment. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998 Nov;10 (4): 426–432. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.4.426 9813788

38. Cummings JL. Cognitive and behavioral heterogeneity in Alzheimer’s disease: seeking the neurological basis. Neurobiol Aging. 2000 Nov; 21(6): 845–861. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(00)00183-4 11124429

39. Torralva T, Roca M, Gleichgerrch E, López P, Manes F. INECO Frontal Screening (IFS): A brief, sensitive, and specific tool to assess executive functions in dementia. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2009 Jul 28;15(05):777. doi: 10.1017/s1355617709990415 19635178

40. Moreira HS, Costa AS, Castro SL, Lima CF, Vicente SG. Assessing Executive Dysfunction in Neurodegenerative Disorders: A Critical Review of Brief Neuropsychological Tools. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017 Nov 9;9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00369 29170636

41. Fiorentino N, Gleichgerrcht E, Roca M, Cetkovich M, Manes F, Torralva T. The INECO Frontal Screening tool differentiates behavioral variant—frontotemporal dementia (bv-FTD) from major depression. Dement Neuropsychol. 2013 Mar;7(1):33–39. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642013DN70100006 29213817

42. Gleichgerrcht E, Roca M, Manes F, Torralva T. Comparing the clinical usefulness of the Institute of Cognitive Neurology (INECO) Frontal Screening (IFS) and the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB) in frontotemporal dementia. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2011 Jul 14;33(9):997–1004. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2011.589375 21923634

43. Custodio N, Herrera-Perez E, Lira D, Roca M, Manes F, Baéz S, Torralva T. Evaluation of the INECO Frontal Screening and the Frontal Assessment Battery in Peruvian patients with Alzheimer's disease and behavioral variant Frontotemporal dementia. eNeurologicalSci, 2016 Dec; 5: 25–29. doi: 10.1016/j.ensci.2016.11.001 29430554

44. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2005 Apr;53(4):695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x 15817019

45. Freitas S, Simões MR, Alves L, Santana I. Montreal Cognitive Assessment: Validation Study for Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer Disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2013;27(1):37–43. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e3182420bfe 22193353

46. Montiel JM, Cecato JF, Bartholomeu D, Martinelli JE. Evaluation of Montreal cognitive assessment for the differential diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease in elderly patients with more than 5 years of schooling: Data from a Brazilian sample. Adv Aging Res. 2013;2(4):121–129. doi: 10.4236/aar.2013.24018

47. Ihnen J, Antivilo A, Muñoz-Neira C, Slachevsky A. Chilean version of the INECO Frontal Screening (IFS-Ch): Psychometric properties and diagnostic accuracy. Dement Neuropsychol. 2013 Mar;7(1):40–47. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642013DN70100007 29213818

48. Sierra Sanjurjo N, Saraniti AB, Gleichgerrcht E, Roca M, Manes F, Torralva T. The IFS (INECO Frontal Screening) and level of education: Normative data. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2018 Feb 12;1–9. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2018.1427096 29432039

49. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. American Psychological Association. 1983; 67(6): 361–370. doi: 10.1037/t03589-000

50. Freitas S, Simões MR, Alves L, Santana I. The Relevance of Sociodemographic and Health Variables on MMSE Normative Data. Appl Neuropsychol Adult. 2014 Dec 22;22(4):311–319. doi: 10.1080/23279095.2014.926455 25531579

51. McKhann GM, Knopman DS, Chertkow H, Hyman BT, Jack CR, Kawas CH, et al. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging and the Alzheimer’s Association workgroup. Alzheimer Dem. 2011; 7: 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.005 21514250

52. Costa AS, Rocha S, Machado Á. Association of retrospective time estimation and severity of cognitive impairment. J Clin Exp Neuropsycho. 2016 May;38(8):853–860. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2016.1167841 27132467

53. Van Cauwenberghe C., Van Broeckhoven C., & Sleegers K. (2016). The genetic landscape of Alzheimer disease: clinical implications and perspectives. Genetics in Medicine, 18(5), 421. doi: 10.1038/gim.2015.117 26312828

54. Reisberg B, Ferris SH, de Leon MJ, Crook T. The Global Deterioration Scale for assessment of primary degenerative dementia. Am J Psychiatr. 1982 Sep;139(9):1136–1139. doi: 10.1176/ajp.139.9.1136 7114305

55. Doody RS, Massman P, Dunn JK. A Method for Estimating Progression Rates in Alzheimer Disease. Arch Neurol. 2001 Mar 1;58(3). doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.3.449 11255449

56. Lyketsos CG, Lopez O, Jones B, Fitzpatrick AL, Breitner J, DeKosky S. Prevalence of Neuropsychiatric Symptoms in Dementia and Mild Cognitive Impairment. JAMA. 2002 Sep 25;288(12):1475. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.12.1475 12243634

57. Modrego PJ, Ferrández J. Depression in Patients With Mild Cognitive Impairment Increases the Risk of Developing Dementia of Alzheimer Type. Arch Neurol. 2004 Aug 1;61(8). doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.8.1290 15313849

58. Aznar S, Knudsen GM. Depression and Alzheimer’s disease: Is stress the initiating factor in a common neuropathological cascade. J Alzheimers Dis. 2011 Feb 26;23: 177–193. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100390 21098983

59. Dubois B., Slachevsky A., Litvan I., & Pillon B. F. A. B. (2000). The FAB: a frontal assessment battery at bedside. Neurology, 55(11), 1621–1626. doi: 10.1212/wnl.55.11.1621 11113214

60. Curran PJ, West SG, Finch JF. The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychol Methods. 1996 Mar;1(1):16–29. doi: 10.1037/1082-989x.1.1.16

61. Statistics Portugal (2012). Censos 2011: Resultados Definitivos–Portugal. [Censos 2011: Definitive Results—Portugal]. Instituto Nacional de Estatística, Lisboa.

62. Franco-Marina F, García-González JJ, Wagner-Echeagaray F, Gallo J, Ugalde O, Sánchez-García S, et al. The Mini-mental State Examination revisited: ceiling and floor effects after score adjustment for educational level in an aging Mexican population. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009 Sep 7;22(01):72. doi: 10.1017/s1041610209990822 19735592

63. Huey ED, Manly JJ, Tang M-X, Schupf N, Brickman AM, Manoochehri M, et al. Course and etiology of dysexecutive MCI in a community sample. Alzheimers Dem. 2013 Nov;9(6):632–639. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.10.014 23452959

64. Hsieh S, Schubert S, Hoon C, Mioshi E, Hodges JR. Validation of the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination III in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2013;36(3–4):242–250. doi: 10.1159/000351671 23949210

65. Kray J, Eber J, Lindenberger U. Age differences in executive functioning across the lifespan: The role of verbalization in task preparation. Acta Psycho. 2004 Feb;115(2–3):143–65. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.001 14962398

66. Isingrini M, Angel L, Fay S, Taconnat L, Lemaire P, Bouazzaoui B. Age-Related Differences in the Reliance on Executive Control in Working Memory: Role of Task Demand. Antal A, editor. PLOS ONE. 2015 Dec 23;10(12):e0145361. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0145361 26700019

67. Appollonio I, Leone M, Isella V, Piamarta F, Consoli T, Villa ML, et al. The Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB): normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci. 2005 Jun;26(2):108–116. doi: 10.1007/s10072-005-0443-4 15995827

68. Branco LD, Cotrena C, Pereira N, Kochhann R, Fonseca RP. Verbal and visuospatial executive functions in healthy elderly: The impact of education and frequency of reading and writing. Dement Neuropsychol. 2014 Jun;8(2):155–161. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642014DN82000011 29213897

69. Roldán-Tapia MD, Cánovas R, León I, García-Garcia J. Cognitive Vulnerability in Aging May Be Modulated by Education and Reserve in Healthy People. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017 Oct 24;9. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00340 29118710

70. Marshall GA, Rentz DM, Frey MT, Locascio JJ, Johnson KA, Sperling RA, Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Executive function and instrumental activities of daily living in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011 May;7(3):300–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2010.04.005 21575871


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 9
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

plice
INSIGHTS from European Respiratory Congress
nový kurz

Současné pohledy na riziko v parodontologii
Autoři: MUDr. Ladislav Korábek, CSc., MBA

Svět praktické medicíny 3/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)

Kardiologické projevy hypereozinofilií
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Petr Němec, Ph.D.

Střevní příprava před kolonoskopií
Autoři: MUDr. Klára Kmochová, Ph.D.

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#