#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Loss of control as a violation of expectations: Testing the predictions of a common inconsistency compensation approach in an inclusionary cyberball game


Autoři: Michael Niedeggen aff001;  Rudolf Kerschreiter aff002;  Katharina Schuck aff001
Působiště autorů: Division of Experimental Psychology and Neuropsychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany aff001;  Division of Social, Organizational, and Economic Psychology, Department of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Berlin, Germany aff002
Vyšlo v časopise: PLoS ONE 14(9)
Kategorie: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221817

Souhrn

Personal control relies on the expectation that events are contingent upon one’s own behavior. A common ‘inconsistency compensation approach’ posits that a violation of expectancies in social interaction triggers aversive arousal and compensatory effort. Following this approach, we tested the hypothesis that interventions affecting participants' decisions violate the expected personal control. In a modified version of the established cyberball paradigm, participants were not excluded, but consistently included. However, their decisions regarding the recipient of a ball throw in the virtual game were occasionally overruled (expectancy violation). We hypothesized that this intervention will trigger a P3 response in event-related brain potentials (ERP). Since this component is related to subjective expectancies, its amplitude was assumed to depend on the frequency of interventions (independent factor: loss of control). Further, we manipulated the vertical position of the participants’ avatar on the computer screen (independent factor: verticality). Building on research showing that verticality is related to the self-assigned power and influences the expected level of control, we hypothesized that the ERP effects of intervention should be more pronounced for participants with avatars in superior position. As predicted, both experimental factors interactively affected the expression of the ERP response: In case of low intervention frequency, P3 amplitudes were significantly pronounced if the participants’ avatar was positioned above as compared to below co-players (high > low self-assigned power). The effect of verticality could be traced back to a lack of adaptation of P3 amplitudes to recurring aversive events. By demonstrating that loss of control triggers ERP effects corresponding to those triggered by social exclusion, this study provides further evidence for a common cognitive mechanism in reactions to aversive events based on an inconsistency in expectancy states.

Klíčová slova:

Research and analysis methods – Bioassays and physiological analysis – Electrophysiological techniques – Brain electrophysiology – Electroencephalography – Event-related potentials – Imaging techniques – Mathematical and statistical techniques – Statistical methods – Analysis of variance – Biology and life sciences – Physiology – Electrophysiology – Neurophysiology – Neuroscience – Brain mapping – Neuroimaging – Cognitive science – Cognition – Psychology – Behavior – Medicine and health sciences – Clinical medicine – Clinical neurophysiology – People and places – Population groupings – Professions – Supervisors – Physical sciences – Mathematics – Statistics – Social sciences


Zdroje

1. Inesi ME, Botti S, Dubois D, Rucker DD, Galinsky AD. Power and Choice: Their Dynamic Interplay in Quenching the Thirst for Personal Control. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(8):1042–8. doi: 10.1177/0956797611413936 WOS:000294709400012. 21705519

2. Burger JM. Desire for Control and Academic-Performance. Can J Behav Sci. 1992;24(2):147–55. doi: 10.1037/h0078716 WOS:A1992HT36800002.

3. MacDonald G, Leary MR. Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physical pain. Psychol Bull. 2005;131(2):202–23. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202 WOS:000227423200004. 15740417

4. Leotti LA, Iyengar SS, Ochsner KN. Born to choose: the origins and value of the need for control. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(10):457–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.001 20817592; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2944661.

5. Kay AC, Gaucher D, Napier JL, Callan MJ, Laurin K. God and the government: Testing a compensatory control mechanism for the support of external systems. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;95(1):18–35. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.1.18 WOS:000257034000002. 18605849

6. Niedeggen M, Kerschreiter R, Hirte D, Weschke S. Being low prepares for being neglected: Verticality affects expectancy of social participation. Psychon Bull Rev. 2017;24(2):574–81. Epub 2016/07/03. doi: 10.3758/s13423-016-1115-5 27368640.

7. Schuck K, Niedeggen M, Kerschreiter R. Violated Expectations in the Cyberball Paradigm: Testing the Expectancy Account of Social Participation With ERP. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9. ARTN 1762 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01762. WOS:000445588300001. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00009

8. Zhou XY, He LN, Yang Q, Lao JP, Baumeister RF. Control Deprivation and Styles of Thinking. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2012;102(3):460–78. doi: 10.1037/a0026316 WOS:000300744000002. 22082059

9. Kerr NL, Levine JM. The detection of social exclusion: Evolution and beyond. Group Dynamics-Theory Research and Practice. 2008;12(1):39–52. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.12.1.39 WOS:000253837000005.

10. Twenge JM, Baumeister RF, DeWall CN, Ciarocco NJ, Bartels JM. Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;92(1):56–66. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.1.56 WOS:000243249300005. 17201542

11. Williams KD. Ostracism. Annual review of psychology. 2007;58:425–52. Epub 2006/09/14. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641 16968209.

12. Narayanan J, Tai K, Kinias Z. Power motivates interpersonal connection following social exclusion. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2013;122(2):257–65. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.08.006 WOS:000328719500014.

13. Magee JC, Smith PK. The social distance theory of power. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. 2013;17(2):158–86. Epub 2013/01/26. doi: 10.1177/1088868312472732 23348983.

14. Quirin M, Meyer F, Heise N, Kuhl J, Kustermann E, Struber D, et al. Neural correlates of social motivation: an fMRI study on power versus affiliation. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;88(3):289–95. Epub 2012/07/31. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.003 22841755.

15. Wang H, Braun C, Enck P. How the brain reacts to social stress (exclusion)—A scoping review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2017;80:80–8. Epub 2017/05/26. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.012 28535967.

16. Syrjamaki AH, Hietanen JK. The effects of social exclusion on processing of social information—A cognitive psychology perspective. Br J Soc Psychol. 2018. Epub 2018/11/28. doi: 10.1111/bjso.12299 30480823.

17. Proulx T, Inzlicht M, Harmon-Jones E. Understanding all inconsistency compensation as a palliative response to violated expectations. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(5):285–91. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.002 WOS:000304026200009. 22516239

18. Muthukrishna M, Henrich J. A problem in theory. Nat Hum Behav. 2019;3(3):221–9. doi: 10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1 30953018.

19. Brehm JW, Cohen AR. Choice and Chance Relative Deprivation as Determinants of Cognitive-Dissonance. J Abnorm Soc Psych. 1959;58(3):383–7. WOS:A1959CCB3400015.

20. Brehm JW, Jones RA. Effect on Dissonance of Surprise Consequences. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1970;6(4):420–&. WOS:A1970H964200003.

21. Park CL. Making sense of the meaning literature: an integrative review of meaning making and its effects on adjustment to stressful life events. Psychol Bull. 2010;136(2):257–301. doi: 10.1037/a0018301 20192563.

22. Oliveira FT, McDonald JJ, Goodman D. Performance monitoring in the anterior cingulate is not all error related: expectancy deviation and the representation of action-outcome associations. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(12):1994–2004. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.1994 17892382.

23. Botvinick MM, Cohen JD, Carter CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(12):539–46. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.003 15556023.

24. Hartgerink CHJ, van Beest I, Wicherts JM, Williams KD. The Ordinal Effects of Ostracism: A Meta-Analysis of 120 Cyberball Studies. Plos One. 2015;10(5). UNSP e0127002 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0127002 WOS:000355319400029. 26023925

25. Zadro L, Williams KD, Richardson R. How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer is sufficient to lower self-reported levels of belonging, control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2004;40(4):560–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.006 ISI:000222248300012.

26. Somerville LH, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM. Anterior cingulate cortex responds differentially to expectancy violation and social rejection. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(8):1007–8. doi: 10.1038/nn1728 16819523.

27. van der Molen MJW, Dekkers LMS, Westenberg PM, van der Veen FM, van der Molen MW. Why don't you like me? Midfrontal theta power in response to unexpected peer rejection feedback. Neuroimage. 2017;146:474–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.045 27566260.

28. Kawamoto T, Nittono H, Ura M. Cognitive, Affective, and Motivational Changes during Ostracism: An ERP, EMG, and EEG Study Using a Computerized Cyberball Task. Neurosci J. 2013;2013:304674. doi: 10.1155/2013/304674 26317090; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4437265.

29. Themanson JR, Schreiber JA, Larsen AD, Dunn KR, Ball AB, Khatcherian SM. The ongoing cognitive processing of exclusionary social events: evidence from event-related potentials. Soc Neurosci. 2015;10(1):55–69. Epub 2014/09/11. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.956899 25204663.

30. Donchin E, Coles MG. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 1988;11:357–74.

31. Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clinical neurophysiology: official journal of the International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2007;118(10):2128–48. Epub 2007/06/19. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019 17573239; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2715154.

32. Picton TW. The P300 wave of the human event-related potential. J Clin Neurophysiol. 1992;9(4):456–79. Epub 1992/10/01. 1464675.

33. Polich J, Margala C. P300 and probability: comparison of oddball and single-stimulus paradigms. International journal of psychophysiology: official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology. 1997;25(2):169–76. Epub 1997/02/01. 9101341.

34. Rosenfeld JP, Biroschak JR, Kleschen MJ, Smith KM. Subjective and objective probability effects on P300 amplitude revisited. Psychophysiology. 2005;42(3):356–9. Epub 2005/06/10. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00283.x 15943689.

35. Gutz L, Kupper C, Renneberg B, Niedeggen M. Processing social participation: an event-related brain potential study. Neuroreport. 2011;22(9):453–8. Epub 2011/05/12. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e3283476b67 21558970.

36. Weschke S, Niedeggen M. ERP effects and perceived exclusion in the Cyberball paradigm: Correlates of expectancy violation? Brain Res. 2015;1624:265–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.038 26236023.

37. Kiat JE, Straley E, Cheadle JE. Why won't they sit with me? An exploratory investigation of stereotyped cues, social exclusion, and the P3b. Soc Neurosci. 2017;12(5):612–25. Epub 2016/08/25. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2016.1223167 27557430.

38. Gutz L, Renneberg B, Roepke S, Niedeggen M. Neural processing of social participation in borderline personality disorder and social anxiety disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2015;124(2):421–31. doi: 10.1037/a0038614 25603358.

39. Weinbrecht A, Niedeggen M, Roepke S, Renneberg B. Feeling excluded no matter what? Bias in the processing of social participation in borderline personality disorder. Neuroimage Clin. 2018;19:343–50. Epub 2018/07/18. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.04.031 30013917; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6044182.

40. Schubert TW. Your highness: vertical positions as perceptual symbols of power. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2005;89(1):1–21. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.1.1 16060739.

41. De Cremer D, Van Dijk E. When and why leaders put themselves first: Leader behaviour in resource allocations as a function of feeling entitled. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2005;35(4):553–63. doi: 10.1002/Ejsp.260 WOS:000230837000007.

42. Schuck Niedeggen, Kerschreiter. Violated Expectations in the Cyberball Paradigm: Testing the Expectancy Account of Social Participation With ERP. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1762. Epub 2018/10/16. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01762 30319487; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6167485.

43. Sawaoka T, Hughes BL, Ambady N. Power Heightens Sensitivity to Unfairness Against the Self. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2015;41(8):1023–35. doi: 10.1177/0146167215588755 WOS:000357803100001. 26048859

44. Dien J, Spencer KM, Donchin E. Parsing the late positive complex: mental chronometry and the ERP components that inhabit the neighborhood of the P300. Psychophysiology. 2004;41(5):665–78. Epub 2004/08/21. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2004.00193.x 15318873.

45. Kopp B, Seer C, Lange F, Kluytmans A, Kolossa A, Fingscheidt T, et al. P300 amplitude variations, prior probabilities, and likelihoods: A Bayesian ERP study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2016;16(5):911–28. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0442-3 27406085.

46. Cislak A, Cichocka A, Wojcik AD, Frankowska N. Power Corrupts, but Control Does Not: What Stands Behind the Effects of Holding High Positions. Pers Soc Psychol B. 2018;44(6):944–57. doi: 10.1177/0146167218757456 WOS:000432109200011. 29484921

47. Sleegers WWA, Proulx T, van Beest I. The social pain of Cyberball: Decreased pupillary reactivity to exclusion cues. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2017;69:187–200. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2016.08.004 WOS:000392774500022.

48. Mars RB, Debener S, Gladwin TE, Harrison LM, Haggard P, Rothwell JC, et al. Trial-by-trial fluctuations in the event-related electroencephalogram reflect dynamic changes in the degree of surprise. J Neurosci. 2008;28(47):12539–45. Epub 2008/11/21. 28/47/12539 [pii]doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2925-08.2008 19020046.

49. Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A. Gpower: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers. 1996;28:1–11.

50. Peirce JW. PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. J Neurosci Methods. 2007;162(1–2):8–13. Epub 2007/01/27. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017 17254636; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2018741.

51. Marks DF. Visual Imagery Differences in Recall of Pictures. British journal of psychology. 1973;64(Feb):17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x. WOS:A1973P379800003. 4742442

52. Lim S, Reeves B. Being in the Game: Effects of Avatar Choice and Point of View on Psychophysiological Responses During Play. Media Psychol. 2009;12(4):348–70. doi: 10.1080/15213260903287242 WOS:000274637900002.

53. Williams KD, Cheung CK, Choi W. Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored over the Internet. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000;79(5):748–62. Epub 2000/11/18. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.79.5.748 11079239.

54. Lammers J, Stoker JI, Stapel DA. Differentiating Social and Personal Power: Opposite Effects on Stereotyping, but Parallel Effects on Behavioral Approach Tendencies. Psychol Sci. 2009;20(12):1543–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02479.x WOS:000272163600018. 19906122

55. Themanson JR, Khatcherian SM, Ball AB, Rosen PJ. An event-related examination of neural activity during social interactions. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2013;8(6):727–33. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss058 WOS:000323454700016. 22577169

56. Gray HM, Ambady N, Lowenthal WT, Deldin P. P300 as an index of attention to self-relevant stimuli. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2004;40(2):216–24. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00092-1 WOS:000220023000007.

57. Weschke S, Niedeggen M. Target and Non-Target Processing during Oddball and Cyberball: A Comparative Event-Related Potential Study. PLoS One. 2016;11(4). ARTN e0153941 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153941 WOS:000374898500103. 27100787

58. Galinsky AD, Magee JC, Gruenfeld DH, Whitson JA, Liljenquist KA. Power Reduces the Press of the Situation: Implications for Creativity, Conformity, and Dissonance. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2008;95(6):1450–66. doi: 10.1037/a0012633 WOS:000261205900015. 19025295

59. Hogeveen J, Inzlicht M, Obhi SS. Power Changes How the Brain Responds to Others. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2014;143(2):755–62. doi: 10.1037/a0033477 WOS:000349768000025. 23815455

60. Ghotekar GS, Shaikh AC, Muthukrishnan M. Transition-Metal-Free Benzannulation of Tricarbonyl Derivatives with Arynes: Access to 1,3-Dinaphthol Precursors for the Synthesis of Rhodamine Dye Analogues. J Org Chem. 2019. doi: 10.1021/acs.joc.8b02560 30628774.

61. Hudac CM. Social priming modulates the neural response to ostracism: a new exploratory approach. Soc Neurosci. 2018:1–15. Epub 2018/04/11. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2018.1463926 29634405.

62. Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD. Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(7):294–300. Epub 2004/07/10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.010 [pii]. 15242688.

63. Warburton WA, Williams KD, Cairns DR. When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2006;42(2):213–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.005 WOS:000235696700007.

64. Slepian ML, Masicampo EJ, Ambady N. Cognition From on High and Down Low: Verticality and Construal Level. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015;108(1):1–17. doi: 10.1037/a0038265 WOS:000348048200001. 25603367


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2019 Číslo 9
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

plice
INSIGHTS from European Respiratory Congress
nový kurz

Současné pohledy na riziko v parodontologii
Autoři: MUDr. Ladislav Korábek, CSc., MBA

Svět praktické medicíny 3/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)

Kardiologické projevy hypereozinofilií
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Petr Němec, Ph.D.

Střevní příprava před kolonoskopií
Autoři: MUDr. Klára Kmochová, Ph.D.

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#