Cultural transmission in a food preparation task: The role of interactivity, innovation and storytelling
Autoři:
Lucas M. Bietti aff001; Adrian Bangerter aff001; Dominique Knutsen aff003; Eric Mayor aff001
Působiště autorů:
Institute of Work and Organizational Psychology, University of Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel, Switzerland
aff001; Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Télécom Paris, Institut Interdisciplinaire de l’innovation, UMR 9217, Paris, France
aff002; Univ. Lille, CNRS, CHU Lille, UMR 9193—SCALab—Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, Lille, France
aff003
Vyšlo v časopise:
PLoS ONE 14(9)
Kategorie:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221278
Souhrn
Interactive conversation drives the transmission of cultural information in small groups and large networks. In formal (e.g. schools) and informal (e.g. home) learning settings, interactivity does not only allow individuals and groups to faithfully transmit and learn new knowledge and skills, but also to boost cumulative cultural evolution. Here we investigate how interactivity affects performance, teaching, learning, innovation and chosen diffusion mode (e.g. instructional discourse vs. storytelling) of previously acquired information in a transmission chain experiment. In our experiment, participants (n = 288) working in 48 chains with three generations of pairs had to learn and complete a collaborative food preparation task (ravioli-making), and then transmit their experience to a new generation of participants in an interactive and non-interactive condition. Food preparation is a real-world task that it is taught and learned across cultures and transmitted over generations in families and groups. Pairs were defined as teachers or learners depending on their role in the transmission chain. The number of good exemplars of ravioli each pair produced was taken as measurement of performance. Contrary to our expectations, the results did not reveal that (1) performance increased over generations or that (2) interactivity in transmission sessions promoted increased performance. However, the results showed that (3) interactivity promoted the transmission of more information from teachers to learners; (4) increased quantity of information transmission from teachers led to higher performance in learners; (5) higher performance generations introduced more innovations in transmission sessions; (6) learners applied those transmitted innovations to their performance which made them persist over generations; (7) storytelling was specialized for the transmission of non-routine, unexpected information. Our findings offer new insights on how interactivity, innovation and storytelling affect the cultural transmission of complex collaborative tasks.
Klíčová slova:
People and places – Population groupings – Professions – Teachers – Biology and life sciences – Neuroscience – Cognitive science – Cognitive psychology – Learning – Human learning – Learning and memory – Psychology – Behavior – Evolutionary biology – Evolutionary processes – Evolutionary emergence – Social sciences – Sociology – Culture – Cultural evolution – Education – Teaching methods
Zdroje
1. O'Brien MJ, Shennan SJ, editors. Innovation in cultural systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2009.
2. Mesoudi A, O’Brien M.J. The learning and transmission of hierarchical cultural recipes. Biol. Theory. 2008; 3: 63–72.
3. Caldwell CA, Millen AE Social learning mechanisms and cumulative cultural evolution: Is imitation necessary? Psychol. Sci. 2009; 20: 1478–1483. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02469.x 19891752
4. Kendal RL, Boogert NJ, Rendell L, Laland KN, Webster M, Jones PL. Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2018; 22 (7): 651–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003 29759889
5. Kline MA. How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behav. Brain Sci. 2015; 8. e31. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X14000090
6. Morgan TJH, Uomini NT, Rendell LE, Chouinard-Thuly L, Street SE, Lewis HM et al. Experimental evidence for the co-evolution of hominin tool-making teaching and language. Nat. Commun. 2015; 6. 6029. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7029 25585382
7. Lombao D, Guardiola M, Mosquera M. Teaching to make stone tools: new experimental evidence supporting a technological hypothesis for the origins of language. Sci. Rep. 2017; 14394. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14322-y
8. Hochberg ME, Marquet PA, Boyd R, Wagner A. Innovation: an emerging focus from cells to societies. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, Bil Sci. 2017; 372(1735): pii: 20160414. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0414 29061887
9. Bavelas JB, Coates L, Johnson T. Listeners as co-narrators. J. Pers, Soc. Psychol. 2000; 79: 941–942.
10. Dunbar RIM. Gossip in evolutionary perspective. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 2004; 8: 100–110.
11. Maswood R, Rajaram S. Social transmission of false memory in small groups and large networks. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12348
12. Tan R, Fay N. Cultural transmission in the laboratory: Agent interaction improves the intergenerational transfer of information. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2011; 32: 399–406.
13. Fay N, Ellison TM, Tylén K. Fusaroli R, Walker B, Garrod S. Cultural ratchet to a social artefact: The cumulative cultural evolution of a language game. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2018; 39: 300–309.
14. Clark HH. Using language. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press; 1996.
15. Clark HH, Wilkes-Gibbs D. Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition. 1986: 22, 1‑39. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90010-7. 3709088
16. Clark HH, Schaefer EF. Contributing to discourse. Cogn. Sci. 1989; 13: 259‑294. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1302_7.
17. Clark HH, Brennan SE. Grounding in communication. In: Resnick LB, Levine JM, Teasley SD, editors. Perspectives on socially shared cognition. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1991, pp.127–149.
18. Knutsen D, Ros C, Le Bigot L. Generating references in naturalistic face-to-face and phone mediated dialogue settings. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2016; 8: 796‑818. doi: 10.1111/tops.12218 27541074
19. Kraut RE, Fussell SR, Siegel J. Visual information as a conversational resource in collaborative physical tasks. Hum. Comput. Interact. 2003; 18: 13–49. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327051HCI1812_2
20. Caldwell CA, Atkinson M, Renner E. Experimental approaches to studying cumulative cultural evolution. Curr. Dir. Psychol, Sci, 2016; 25: 191–195.
21. Mesoudi A, Whiten A. The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, Bil Sci. 2008; 363, 3489–3501.
22. Bartlett F. Remembering. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1932.
23. Zwirner E, Thornton A. Cognitive requirements of cumulative culture: teaching is useful but not essential. Sci. Rep. 2015; 5: 16781. doi: 10.1038/srep16781 26606853
24. Caldwell CA, Millen A. Experimental models for testing hypotheses about cumulative cultural evolution. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2008; 29: 165–171.
25. Bietti LM, Tilston OER, Bangerter A. Storytelling as adaptive collective sensemaking. Top. Cogn. Sci. 2018; https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12358.
26. Mandelbaum J. (2013). Storytelling in conversation. In: Sidnell J, Stivers T, editors. Handbook of conversation analysis. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2013, pp. 492–508.
27. Labov W, Waletzky J. Narrative analysis. In: Helm J. editor. Essays on the verbal and visual arts. Seattle: University of Washington Press; 1976, pp. 12–44.
28. Bruner JS. Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1990
29. Scalise Sugiyama M. Food, foragers, and folklore: The role of narrative in human subsistence. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2001; 22: 221–240
30. Brown DE. Human Universals. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1991.
31. Smith D, Schlaepfer P, Major K, Dyble M, Page AE., Thompson J, et al. Cooperation and the evolution of hunter-gatherer storytelling. Nat. Comm. 2017; 8: 1–19. 1853. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02036-8.
32. Garfield ZH, Garfield MJ, Hewlett BS. A cross-cultural analysis of hunter-gatherer social learning. In: Terashima H, Hewlett B, editors. Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers. Tokyo, Japan: Springer; 2016, pp. 19–34. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-4-431-55997-9_2
33. Silva KG, Correa-Chávez M, Rogoff B. Mexican-heritage children's attention and learning from interactions directed to others. Child Dev. 2010; 81: 898–912. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01441.x 20573112
34. Dessalles JL. (2010). Have you anything unexpected to say? The human propensity to communicate surprise and its role in the emergence of language. In: Smith ADM, Schouwstra M, de Boer B, Smith K. editors. The evolution of language. Proceedings of the 8th international conference (Evolang8—Utrecht). Singapore: World Scientific; 2010, pp.99–106. https://doi.org/10.1142/9789814295222_0013
35. Bietti LM, Bangerter A, Mayor E. The interactive shaping of social learning in transmission chains. In: Gunzelmann G, Howes A, Tenbrink T, Davelaar E, editors. Proc. 39th Annual Conf. Cognitive Science Soc. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society; 2017, pp.1641–1646.
36. Aiello LC, Wheeler P. The expensive-tissue hypothesis—The brain and the digestive system in human and primate evolution. Curr Anthropol. 1995;36:199–221. doi: 10.1086/204350
37. Carmody RN, Wrangham RW. The energetic significance of cooking. J Hum Evol. 2009; 57:379–391. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.02.011 19732938
38. McCabe M, de Waal Malefyt T. Creativity and cooking: Motherhood, agency and social change in everyday life. J Consum Cult. 2015; 15: 48–65.
39. Farmer N, Touchton-Leonard K, Ross A. Psychosocial benefits of cooking interventions: a systematic review. Health Educ Behav. 45: 167–80. doi: 10.1177/1090198117736352 29121776
40. Wrangham RW, Jones JH, Laden G, Pilbeam D, Conklin-Brittain N. The raw and the stolen—Cooking and the ecology of human origins. Curr Anthropol. 1999;40:567–594. doi: 10.1086/300083 10539941
41. Utter J, Larson N, Laska MN, Winkler M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Self perceived cooking skills in emerging adulthood predict better dietary behaviors and intake 10 years later: a longitudinal study. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2018; 50: 494–500. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2018.01.021 29525525
42. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1985.
43. Derex M, Boyd R, The foundations of the human cultural niche. Nat. Commun. 2015; 6, 8398. doi: 10.1038/ncomms9398 26400015
44. Bangerter A. Identifying individual and collective acts of remembering in task-related communication. Discourse Proces. 2000; 30: 237–264.
45. Ramstead MJD, Veissière SPL, Kirmayer LJ. Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Front Psychol. 2016; 7:1090. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090 27507953
46. Caldwell CA., Renner E, Atkinson M. Human teaching and cumulative cultural evolution. Rev. Phil. Psych. 2018; 9: 751. doi: 10.1007/s13164-017-0346-3 30595765
47. McGuigan N, Burdett E., Burgess V, Dean L, Lucas A, Vale G et al. Innovation and social transmission in experimental micro-societies: exploring the scope of cumulative culture in young children. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, Bil Sci. 2017; 5;372(1735). pii: 20160425. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0425 29061897
48. Bangerter A, Mayor E, Pekarek Doehler S. Reported speech in conversational storytelling during nursing shift handover meetings. Discourse Proces. 2011; 48: 183–214. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2010.519765.
49. Boyd B. On the origins of stories: Evolution, cognition and fiction. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 2009.
50. Currie A, Sterelny K. (2017). In defence of story‐telling. Stud. His. Philos. Sci. A. 2017; 62: 14–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.shpsa.2017.03.003.
51. Dunbar R. Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber and Faber; 2010.
52. Milton H, Charbonneau M. Cumulative culture in the laboratory: methodological and theoretical challenges. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B, Bil Sci. 2018; 285(1879): 20180677. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0677
Článek vyšel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 9
- S diagnostikou Parkinsonovy nemoci může nově pomoci AI nástroj pro hodnocení mrkacího reflexu
- Je libo čepici místo mozkového implantátu?
- Pomůže v budoucnu s triáží na pohotovostech umělá inteligence?
- AI může chirurgům poskytnout cenná data i zpětnou vazbu v reálném čase
- Nová metoda odlišení nádorové tkáně může zpřesnit resekci glioblastomů
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- Graviola (Annona muricata) attenuates behavioural alterations and testicular oxidative stress induced by streptozotocin in diabetic rats
- CH(II), a cerebroprotein hydrolysate, exhibits potential neuro-protective effect on Alzheimer’s disease
- Comparison between Aptima Assays (Hologic) and the Allplex STI Essential Assay (Seegene) for the diagnosis of Sexually transmitted infections
- Assessment of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase activity using CareStart G6PD rapid diagnostic test and associated genetic variants in Plasmodium vivax malaria endemic setting in Mauritania
Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova
Všechny kurzy