Children with HIV: A scoping review of auditory processing skills
Authors:
Gouwa Dawood aff001; Daleen Klop aff001; Elrietha Olivier aff001; Haley Elliott aff001; Mershen Pillay aff002
Authors place of work:
Division of Speech, Language and Hearing Therapy, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, Stellenbosch University, Tygerberg, Cape Town, South Africa
aff001; Discipline of Speech-Language Pathology, School of Health Sciences, University of KwaZulu-Natal, Westville, Durban, South Africa
aff002
Published in the journal:
PLoS ONE 14(9)
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0221573
Summary
Introduction
Auditory processing disorders can negatively affect academic performance in children. They can result from a number of aetiologies, including the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV). Although studies in paediatrics are limited, research suggests that HIV-infected children display poorer auditory processing skills than uninfected children.
Methods
The aims of this study were to scan the peer-reviewed literature on auditory processing skills in HIV-infected children, to describe how auditory processing was tested, how auditory processing skills were reported, and to identify gaps in current evidence. This systematic scoping review was conducted using a modified version of Arksey and O’Malley’s framework. Key words comprised ‘HIV’, ‘auditory processing’, ‘hearing’ and ‘child’. Electronic databases were searched for relevant articles published from 1 January 2000 to 30 April 2018, and reference lists of included studies were pearled. Two researchers reviewed the articles and extracted data on sample descriptors, auditory processing testing procedures, and auditory processing skills. A third author collated the results and resolved discrepancies. The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association description of auditory processing skills framed the analysis.
Results
Five articles were included in this review (three from Brazil, one each from Mexico and Tanzania). Samples, and methods of testing were heterogeneous. Three studies reported on localization abilities, while gap detection thresholds, performance on dichotic tasks and speech discrimination scores were reported in one article each. No one study tested all areas of auditory processing skills and there was limited information about the auditory processing skills required for learning.
Conclusion
This review highlighted the current sparse evidence-base for auditory processing in HIV-infected children. It identified the need to standardise testing procedures, measures of auditory processing skills, and sample selection.
Keywords:
Biology and life sciences – Organisms – Research and analysis methods – Database and informatics methods – Neuroscience – People and places – Population groupings – Medicine and health sciences – Microbiology – Medical microbiology – Microbial pathogens – Pathology and laboratory medicine – Pathogens – Physiology – Pediatrics – infectious diseases – Viral pathogens – Immunodeficiency viruses – HIV – Retroviruses – Lentivirus – viruses – RNA viruses – Age groups – children – Families – Viral diseases – HIV infections – Otorhinolaryngology – Database searching – Otology – Hearing disorders – deafness – Sensory physiology – Auditory system – Sensory systems – Pediatric infections
Introduction
Effective drug regimens have resulted in decreasing mortality rates from the human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) [1]. This has shifted attention to the impact of HIV on the developmental and educational outcomes of the approximately 1.8 million children, who are currently living with HIV [2]. HIV-infected children have been shown to perform poorer academically than their non-infected peers, with 40% of infected children aged between six and 12 years, being in a lower grade than is appropriate for their age [3]. It has therefore been suggested that HIV-infected children be recognised as a group with distinct educational needs [4]. However, in order to address their specific educational needs, research is required to describe the associated conditions affecting educational achievement.
Research has shown an association between hearing loss and HIV in the paediatric population with prevalence rates ranging from 6% to 84.4% [3,5–13]. Hearing loss in this population may be caused by the virus itself, opportunistic diseases and associated treatment regimens [14]. Hearing loss, which leads to reduced access to auditory information, significantly impacts a child’s ability to listen and learn in a mainstream classroom environment [15]. However, listening difficulties may not only be associated with hearing loss but more subtly, a child can have normal hearing but an impaired ability to process auditory information [16].
Auditory processing can be described as assigning meaning to what has been heard [17]. Discreet auditory processing skills form the foundation for listening [18]. These skills include: sound localization and lateralization, auditory discrimination, auditory temporal processing, auditory pattern processing, dichotic listening, auditory performance in competing acoustic signals, and auditory performance with degraded acoustic signals [19]. Whilst there is disagreement in the international literature about how or where auditory processing difficulties originate, for children with these deficits, the ability to function in a typical class environment is diminished [20].
Overall, research within HIV-infected populations has primarily reported on peripheral hearing loss [21], with few studies reporting on auditory processing. Although findings suggestive of central auditory deficits have been reported [5,8,22,23], these studies looked at the integrity of the auditory pathway rather than specific auditory processing skills. Matas et al [24], however, found that HIV-infected children were poorer at localizing sound than uninfected children. These findings have important implications for the paediatric HIV population, as disorders affecting auditory processing, in the absence of hearing loss, have been associated with poorer educational outcomes [25].
Objective
The aim of this review was to systematically scan the published literature to identify papers of any research design that reported on auditory processing skills in the HIV-infected paediatric population.
Methods
Study design
A scoping review entails mapping key concepts and summarising available evidence of a particular research area [26,27]. A scoping review was the most appropriate design to address the objectives of this study, because so little is known about this area [28].
Protocol registration
No protocol was registered for this scoping review as it was part of a bigger study that required the protocol to be registered by the Health Research Ethics Committee, Stellenbosch University.
Reporting standard
This paper uses the PRISMA-ScR checklist [29] as a reporting standard. The review was conducted according to the framework proposed by Arksey and O’Malley [26], which was further developed by Levac, Colquhoun and O’Brien [27].
Eligibility criteria
The search was conducted from 1 January 2000 until 30 April 2018. A PIO search framework was set, where P (patient) reflected children up to 18 years with HIV, I (intervention) was the assessment measure(s) for auditory processing skill(s), and O (outcome) was the auditory processing skill. Studies that included both adults and children were excluded. The inclusion and exclusion criteria are provided in Table 1.
Information sources and search
A comprehensive search strategy was designed in collaboration with a librarian from Stellenbosch University Library (Cape Town, South Africa). The databases searched included EBSCOhost research, Pubmed, Web of Science and Scopus. Within the EBSCOhost research database the following databases were accessed: Academic Search Premier, Africa Wide Information, CINAHL, Health Source: Nursing/Academic edition & MEDLINE.
These databases were searched using a combination of keywords including: HIV, auditory processing, hearing and child. The keywords or MeSH (Medical Subject Headings) terms were adapted according to the indexing of the database. While HIV is often associated with AIDS in the literature, preliminary testing of the search strategy identified that the inclusion in the search of the term ‘AIDS’ identified many irrelevant articles (e.g. where aids referred to assistive devices). This term was then removed from the search on the understanding that its absence in connection with HIV would not diminish the search parameters.
Study selection
Studies identified by the search were first screened for duplicates, then for relevance. A three-step process was followed: the title was first checked for articles that were clearly not relevant for the purposes of the review (e.g. Management of therapeutic hypothermia for neonatal hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in a tertiary centre in South Africa was excluded, as it was not relevant to the focus of the review). Of the retained articles after this step, the abstracts were then read and further exclusions were made in line with the exclusion criteria (e.g. Auditory Impairment in HIV-infected individuals in Tanzania was excluded as it related to the adult population and did not report on a specific auditory processing skill). Articles were then read in full text, and retained if they focused on measurements of auditory processing skills in children who were HIV positive (Fig 1). Reference lists of the retained articles were then searched for additional studies that may not have been identified by the primary search. As a final step, an international expert on auditory processing was consulted on whether the auditory skills identified from each article, were correctly identified.
Data collection process
A data extraction sheet (Table 2) was developed and used to extract data from included articles. The data collection sheet included: author, year, study design, level of evidence, study setting, description of the sample, assessment measures used in each study, auditory processing skill as described by the ASHA [19] and key findings. All stages of the review were conducted by two authors (EO & HE) who worked independently. Findings were then compared, and any discrepancies were discussed and resolved by EO and HE. A third author (GD), who also collated the results, was consulted when a discrepancy could not be resolved.
Hierarchy of evidence
Study design was classified according to the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) levels of evidence [30].
Assessment of risk of bias
Methodological quality of included studies was not evaluated due to the scoping nature of the review [29].
Synthesis of results
Data was summarized for currency, country of origin, method of assessing auditory processing skills and key outcome measures using tables, figures, numerical analysis and narrative synthesis, as recommended by Arksey and O’Malley [26].
Additional analyses
By examining the scope of research described in each article, this enabled the authors to identify gaps in literature and areas for further research.
Results
Study selection
Fig 1 outlines the study selection process. A total of 678 articles were found in the selected databases. After titles were screened for relevance and duplicates were removed, 108 articles remained. Abstracts of these articles were then screened, according to the inclusion and exclusion criteria (Table 1), and resulted in 42 articles being included for full text review. Following full text review, 37 articles were excluded as they did not meet inclusion and exclusion criteria (Table 1). The remaining five articles [5,23,24,31,32] were retained for this review. Table 2 summarises key characteristics of each article. No additional articles were found during the search of relevant reference lists.
Study characteristics
The studies were all cross-sectional designs, whose subjects ranged in age from 1 month to 16 years. Although the study sample sizes ranged from 15 to 244 children, auditory processing skills were not measured in all the subjects. Three of the studies included control groups [23,24,32], while two studies reported case series [5,31]. The studies were all conducted in urban settings, in developing countries, with three of the studies based in Brazil (South America) and the other two studies based respectively in Tanzania (Africa) and Mexico (North America). Table 2 provides a summary of the included studies, and Table 3 summarises the reference population and the sample.
Synthesis of the results
Sampling issues
The five papers reported on four datasets, with Matas et al (2000, 2008) reporting on different aspects of the same sample. The study samples differed in selection criteria, age and health status. Although four papers reported on the sampling location, information on how children were selected for study participation was notably absent in all studies. Overall, the included papers report on 185 HIVP children (cases), of a total of 425 children. All cases were on ART. Within the 240 non-HIVP (control) children were 78 serum-reverted children and 30 HIV exposed children, who were treated separately in subgroup analysis (Matas et al 2000, 2008). Maro et al (2016) recruited 113 HIVN (control) children who were family members of cases (Maro et al 2016). Whilst four studies excluded children with comorbid medical conditions, Maro et al (2016) included 31 HIVP and 3 HIVN children with TB histories. The overall sample age ranged from one month to 18 years, however it was not possible from the available information to estimate an average sample age. On the available data, it might be presumed that the majority of children were younger than 10 years. Palacios et al (2008) reported on 23 Mexican cases (average age 4.5 years); Matas et al (2000, 2008) reported on a dataset of 143 children born to HIVP mothers (all children being younger than 2.5 years); Maro et al (2016) reported on 244 children (cases and controls) of average age 10.1 years, and Romero et al (2017) reported on 15 cases aged 8 or 9 years. Sample heterogeneity potentially underpins the lack of consistency in the findings of this review.
Auditory processing testing procedures
The studies described different test procedures, namely Behavioural Observation Audiometry (size of response, timing of response, attention to sound, lateralization, localization in vertical plane, cochlea-palpebral reflex) (Matas et al 2000, 2008); speech discrimination (Palacios et al 2017); gap detection thresholds (Maro et al 2016); and Staggered Spondaic Words, Simplified Auditory Processing Test (sound localization in 5 directions, memory for verbal sounds, memory for nonverbal sounds) (Romero et al 2017).
Auditory processing skills
Moreover, the test procedures described in the studies assessed different auditory processing skills, Four clusters of skills, described below, were identified during data extraction: (1) binaural interaction (Matas et al 2000, 2008, Romaro 2017); (2) auditory discrimination (Palacios et al 2017); (3) auditory temporal processing (Maro et al 2016) and (4) binaural integration (Romero et al, 2017).
Binaural interaction: Localisation, as a measure of binaural interaction, was assessed in three studies [24,31,32]. A higher occurrence of abnormal findings in the HIV-infected group, compared to the two uninfected groups, was reported by Matas et al [24,32] while Romero et al [31] reported that localisation difficulties occurred in less than 30% of their participants.
Auditory discrimination: The only study that assessed auditory discrimination skills [5], reported that abnormal findings were observed in four children in a sample of nine. The origin of the deficits were attributed to conductive involvement (n = 2), cochlear involvement (n = 1) and central involvement (n = 1).
Auditory temporal processing: Maro et al [23] reported on gap detection thresholds as the outcome measure for auditory temporal processing. The authors reported that the mean and median gap detection thresholds, between children infected with HIV (n = 48) and uninfected children (n = 19) were not significantly different.
Binaural integration: One study assessed binaural integration by using the Staggered Spondiac Word Test (SSW) [31]. Although 87% (13 of the 15 participants) of children presented with difficulties, these difficulties were attributed to problems in the areas of attention, memory and auditory figure-ground skills rather than only problems with binaural integration.
Discussion
This is the first scoping review that we know of, that describes the volume and nature of research relating to auditory processing skills in HIV-infected children. We believe that our scoping review search strategy was sufficiently comprehensive that we identified all available evidence in this area. The strength of the scoping review was enhanced by having two independent researchers undertake the screening and extraction steps. Moreover, the scoping nature of the review enabled the identification of all available literature in this area without the restraints of seeking specific research designs or methodological quality. It thus provided a comprehensive overview of the literature currently available on the topic.
From the limited available literature, there is consistent evidence of deficits in the auditory processing skills that were assessed in HIV-infected children [5,23,24,31,32]. The review highlights that the published research is limited in numbers of studies (five), study designs (cross-sectional), geographical settings (urban, developing countries).
A possible reason for the limited research on auditory processing in the HIV-infected paediatric population, is the lack of consensus between researchers on definitions and/or descriptions of auditory processing [33]. Various definitions have been postulated with the definition by the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [19] being the most widely cited. According to this definition, (Central) Auditory Processing is “the perceptual processing of auditory information in the CNS and the neurobiologic activity that underlies that processing and gives rise to electrophysiologic auditory potentials”. More recently, the British Society of Audiology [34] has described auditory perception as being “the awareness of acoustic stimuli, forming the basis for subsequent action” and “results from both sensory activation (via the ear) and neural processing that integrates this ‘bottom up’ information with activity in other brain systems (e.g. vision, attention, memory)”.
The literature identified in this review identified limitations in the scope of assessment measures for auditory processing skills. The use of assessment measures as a reflection of a specific skill, is complex. A gold standard for the assessment of auditory processing disorders does not exist, although various assessment guidelines recommend that a test battery approach be used and that an auditory processing deficit cannot be diagnosed on the basis of one test, measuring a single auditory processing skill [19,35–38]. The American Speech-Language-Hearing Association [19] lists various categories of central auditory tests that may be considered for the assessment of central auditory processing including: auditory discrimination tests, auditory temporal processing, dichotic speech tests, monaural low-redundancy speech test, binaural interaction tests, electroacoustic measures and electrophysiologic measures. Despite the variety of assessment measures available, four of the five studies included in the review, only reported on measures that were easily quantifiable and not language-dependant, such as auditory discrimination, localization and gap detection thresholds.
Auditory temporal processing do not appear to be affected by HIV [23]. Maro et al. [23] did not report significant differences in gap detection thresholds between HIV-infected and uninfected children. Furthermore, similar performances were reported for HIV-infected children regardless of the type of ART being used or the time delay before initiation of treatment [23]. A possible reason for the lack of statistically significant results was the small sample size [39] as very few children could complete the task due to the complexity of the instructions. Despite similar results being reported for adults when HIV-infected and uninfected individuals were compared [40], within the group of HIV-infected adults, persons using antiretroviral therapy (ART) performed significantly poorer than persons not using medication [40]. Based on the findings of this adult study, a possible explanation for deficits in gap detection is the use of ART, rather than the virus itself. However, the two groups that were compared in the adult study also differed in term of mean age and history of tuberculosis [40] and both these factors have been associated with auditory dysfunction [41,42].
Research suggests that HIV may affect auditory discrimination. The only study that used this skill as an outcome measure, reported that 44% of the sample presented with deficits [5]. However, this study was a case series with a small sample size (nine participants). Although the authors differentiated between conductive, cochlear and central impairment; the inclusion of children with hearing loss may have increased the prevalence of abnormal findings as hearing loss has been associated with impaired speech perception abilities [43].
Binaural interaction skills appear to be impaired in HIV-infected children. This can be seen in deficits in localizing abilities [24,31,32]. However, the extent to which these abilities are affected is not known as one of the studies was a case series [31] which provides limited information about causality and the pattern of the auditory processing deficits [39]. The other studies were case-control studies [24,32] that did not report on the statistical significance of the observed differences. A factor that may have contributed to the prevalence of impaired localizing skills is age, as two of the studies [24,32] assessed babies and infants (interrogating the same sample of children born to HIVP mothers). Muir et al. [44] reported that the development of localizing skills followed a u-shaped trajectory. According to this trajectory, a neonate’s response to sound decreases between the ages of one and three months with a greater, more accurate response being seen from four to five months of age [44].
Binaural integration is reported to be abnormal in HIV-infected children. The findings reported by the one study that assessed performance on a dichotic speech test [31] are consistent with the findings reported for a study conducted in HIV-infected adults [45]. However, when considering the well-documented neurocognitive effects of HIV [46,47], it is difficult to comment on whether it is a pure binaural integration deficit as this skill is so intimately entwined with executive functions, in particular attention and memory [48].
The biologic sequelae of HIV have been well-documented in children, and the studies included in this review provide insights into a few auditory processing skills that are affected in HIV-infected children. These studies use methodologies that answer questions related to the organic nature of HIV and provide information on the deficits that may be associated with HIV. However, when one considers the chronic nature of the virus, these methodologies may need to be supplemented with measures to answer questions related to functional outcomes of HIV. In order to provide appropriate educational services, identifying deficits that may affect learning is not enough, as understanding how these deficits affect the way the individual functions in their learning environment, is also needed.
Limitations
The review findings highlight the scarcity of research in this area. We were concerned that the inclusion of only English language studies may have introduced bias toward countries from Europe and North America, however, all the articles were from countries whose official languages were not English. The exclusion of conference abstracts and other grey literature may have also introduced publication bias; however, this literature may not have provided the detail that we required in order to describe research in the area.
Conclusion
This systematic scoping review identified only five studies on auditory processing skills in HIV-infected children and highlights the paucity of research in the area. This evidence-base is inconclusive regarding the association between HIV and auditory processing difficulties as the studies did not necessarily assess the same auditory processing skill and findings were, therefore not comparable. In order to better understand the impact of the virus on learning and to justify the inclusion of auditory processing assessments in the basic health and educational services offered to children infected with HIV, further research of all auditory processing skills is needed. Carefully designed case control studies looking at the time-sequence and relationship between HIV, auditory processing and learning in children should be considered. As the organic consequences of HIV have frequently been demonstrated, it is imperative that future studies include functional outcomes so as to develop a more comprehensive picture of the auditory abilities of this population. Furthermore, research needs to address issues of sampling, sample size calculation and heterogeneity of the population, as well as exerting better controls for maturational aspects of auditory processing, by carefully considering the age of both participants and controls.
Supporting information
S1 Checklist [docx]
PRISMA-ScR checklist.
S1 Dataset [docx]
S1 Dataset.
Zdroje
1. World Health Organization. World Health Statistics 2018: Monitoring health for the SDGs—sustainable development goals. Geneva; 2018.
2. UNAIDS. UNAIDS 2016: Aids Data. Geneva; 2016.
3. Govender R, Eley B, Walker K, Petersen R, Wilmshurst JM. Neurologic and Neurobehavioral Sequelae in Children With Human Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV-1) Infection. J Child Neurol. 2011 Nov 25;26(11):1355–64. doi: 10.1177/0883073811405203 21616924
4. Tikly L. A roadblock to social justice ? An analysis and critique of the South African education Roadmap. Int J Educ Dev. 2011;31:86–94.
5. Palacios GC, Montalvo MS, Fraire MI, Leon E, Alvarez MT, Solorzano F. Audiologic and vestibular findings in a sample of Human Immunodeficiency Virus type-1-infected Mexican children under Highly Active Antiretroviral Therapy. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2008 Nov;72(11):1671–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2008.08.002 18814921
6. Taipale A, Pelkonen T, Taipale M, Pitka A. Otorhinolaryngological findings and hearing in HIV-positive and HIV-negative children in a developing country. 2011;1527–32.
7. Buriti A, dos Santos Oliveira S, Muniz L. Hearing loss in children with HIV / AIDS Perda auditiva em crianças com HIV / AIDS. CoDAS. 2013;25(1):513–20.
8. Matas CG, Santos Filha VAV Dos, Juan KR De, Pinto FR, Gonçalves IC. Audiological manifestations in children and adults with AIDS. Pro Fono. 2010;22(3):269–74. 21103717
9. Chao CK, Czechowicz JA, Messner AH, Alarcón J, Kolevic Roca L, Larragán Rodriguez MM, et al. High prevalence of hearing impairment in HIV-infected Peruvian children. Otolaryngol Neck Surg. 2012 Feb;146(2):259–65.
10. Assuiti LFC, de Melo Lanzoni GM, dos Santos FC, Erdmann AL, Meirelles BHS. Hearing loss in people with HIV/AIDS and associated factors: an integrative review. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2013 Mar;79(2):248–55. 23670333
11. Christopher N, Edward T, Sabrina B-K, Agnes N. The prevalence of hearing impairment in the 6 months– 5 years HIV/AIDS-positive patients attending paediatric infectious disease clinic at Mulago Hospital. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2013 Feb;77(2):262–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.11.013 23211665
12. Torre P, Cook A, Elliott H, Dawood G, Laughton B. Hearing assessment data in HIV-infected and uninfected children of Cape Town, South Africa. AIDS Care. 2015 Aug 3;27(8):1037–41. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2015.1021746 25760238
13. Hrapcak S, Kuper H, Bartlett P, Devendra A, Makawa A, Kim M, et al. Hearing Loss in HIV-Infected Children in Lilongwe, Malawi. Sokolowski B, editor. PLoS One. 2016 Aug 23;11(8):e0161421. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161421 27551970
14. Khoza-Shangase K. HIV / AIDS and auditory function in adults : the need for intensified research in the developing world AJAR. African J AIDS Resaerch. 2010;9(1):1–9.
15. Most T. The effects of degree and type of hearing loss on children’s performance in class. Deaf Educ Int. 2004 Oct;6(3):154–66.
16. Chalfant JC, Scheffeling MA. Central processing dyfunctions in children: A review of the reearch—phase theree of a three-phase project. Bethesda: Public Health Service, U.S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare; 1969. 148 p.
17. Cole E, Flexer C. Children with hearing loss: Developing listening and talking—Birth to Six. 2nd ed. San Diego: Plural Publishing, Inc.; 2011. 434 p.
18. MacIver-Lux K, Lim SR, Rhoades EA, Robertson L, Quayle R, Hönck L. Milestones in Auditory-Verbal Development: Auditory Processing, Speech, Language, Emergent Literacy, Play, and Theory of Mind. In: Estabrooks W, MacIver-Lux K, Rhoades EA, editors. Auditry Verbal Therapy: For young children with hearing loss and ther families, and the practitioners who guide them. San Diego: Plural Publishing, Inc.; 2016. p. 219–61.
19. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. Technical report: (Central) Auditory Processing Disorders. 2005.
20. Lucker JR. Should Children with Auditory Processing Disorders Receive Services in Schools? J Am Acad Spec Educ. 2012;125–35.
21. Ensink RJH, Kuper H. Is hearing impairment associated with HIV? A systematic review of data from low- and middle-income countries. Trop Med Int Heal. 2017;22(12):1493–504.
22. Matas CG, Leite RA, Magliaro FCL, Gonçalves IC. Audiological and electrophysiological evaluation of children with acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (AIDS). Braz J Infect Dis. 2006;10(4):264–8. 17293909
23. Maro II, Fellows AM, Clavier OH, Gui J, Rieke CC, Wilbur JC, et al. Auditory Impairments in HIV-Infected Children. Ear Hear. 2016;37(4):443–51. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000276 26881980
24. Matas CG, Iorio MMCM, Succi RCDM, Cecília M. Auditory Disorders and Acquisition of the Ability to Localize Sound in Children Born to HIV-Positive Mothers. Brazilian J Infect Dis. 2008;12(1):10–4. 18553007
25. Lucker JR. The History of Auditory Processing Disorders in Children. In: Geffner D, Ross-Swain D, editors. Auditory Porcessing Disorders: Assessment, management and treatment. 2nd ed. San Diego: Plural Publishing, Inc.; 2013. p. 33–57.
26. Arksey H, O’Malley L. Scoping studies: towards a methodological framework. Int J Soc Res Methodol. 2005 Feb;8(1):19–32.
27. Levac D, Colquhoun H, O’Brien KK. Scoping studies: advancing the methodology. Implement Sci. 2010 Jan;5(69).
28. Rumrill PD, Fitzgerald SM, Merchant WR. Using scoping literature reviews as a means of understanding and interpreting existing literature. Work. 2010;35(3):399–404. doi: 10.3233/WOR-2010-0998 20364059
29. Tricco AC, Lillie E, Zarin W, O’Brien KK, Colquhoun H, Levac D, et al. PRISMA Extension for Scoping Reviews (PRISMA-ScR): Checklist and Explanation. Ann Intern Med. 2018 Oct 2;169(7):467. doi: 10.7326/M18-0850 30178033
30. Joanna Briggs Institute Levels of Evidence and Grades of Recommendation Working Party. New JBI Levels of evidence. Joanna Briggs Institute. 2013.
31. Romero A, Alfaya L, Gonçales A, Frizzo A, Isaac M. Auditory Alterations in Children Infected by Human Immunodeficiency Virus Verified Through Auditory Processing Test. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2016 Mar 14;21(01):86–91.
32. Matas CG, Sansone AP, Iorio MCM, Succi RCM. Audiological evaluation in children born to HIV positive mothers. Braz J Otorhinolaryngol. 2000;66(4):317–24.
33. McFarland DJ, Cacace AT. Models of central auditory processing abilities and disorders. In: Cacace AT, McFarland DJ, editors. Controversies in central auditory processing disorder. 1st ed. Oxforshire: Plural Publishing, Inc.; 2009. p. 353.
34. British Society of Audiology. Position Statement: Auditory Processing Disorders. 2011.
35. British Society of Audiology. Position Statement and Practice Guidance Auditory Processing Disorder (APD). 2017.
36. De Wit E, Luinge M, Neijenhuis K. Dutch Position Statement Children with Listening Difficulties. [Translated version of The Dutch Position Statement Kinderen met Luisterproblemen]. 2017.
37. The Canadian Interorganizational Steering Group for Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology. Canadian Guidelines on Auditory Processing Disorder in Children and Adults: Assessment and Intervention. 2012.
38. American Academy of Audiology. American Academy of Audiology Clinical Practice Guidelines: Diagnosis, Treatment and Management of Children and Adults with Central Auditory Processing Disorder. 2010.
39. Webb P, Bain C. Essential Epidemiology: An introduction for students and health professionals. 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2011. 1–445 p.
40. Maro II, Moshi N, Clavier OH, Mackenzie T a, Kline-Schoder RJ, Wilbur JC, et al. Auditory Impairments in HIV-Infected Individuals in Tanzania. Ear Hear. 2014;35(3):306–17. doi: 10.1097/01.aud.0000439101.07257.ed 24441742
41. Humes LE. Age-Related Changes in Cognitive and Sensory Processing: Focus on Middle-Aged Adults. Am J Audiol. 2015 Jun;24(2):94–7. doi: 10.1044/2015_AJA-14-0063 25768926
42. Schnippel K, Firnhaber C, Berhanu R, Page-Shipp L, Sinanovic E. Adverse drug reactions during drug-resistant TB treatment in high HIV prevalence settings: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Antimicrob Chemother. 2017 Jul;72(7):1871–9. doi: 10.1093/jac/dkx107 28419314
43. Cai T, McPherson B. Hearing loss in children with otitis media with effusion: a systematic review. Int J Audiol. 2017 Feb 14;56(2):65–76. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2016.1250960 27841699
44. Muir DW, Clifton RK, Clarkson MG. The development of a human auditory localization response: a U-shaped function. Can J Psychol. 1989;43(2):199–216. 2486495
45. Heilman KJ, Harden ER, Weber KM, Cohen M, Porges SW. Atypical autonomic regulation, auditory processing, and affect recognition in women with HIV. Biol Psychol. 2013 Feb;94(1):143–51. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.06.003 23792136
46. Crowell CS, Malee KM, Yogev R, Muller WJ. Neurologic disease in HIV-infected children and the impact of combination antiretroviral therapy. Rev Med Virol. 2014 Sep;24(5):316–31. doi: 10.1002/rmv.1793 24806816
47. Laughton B, Cornell M, Boivin M, Van Rie A. Neurodevelopment in perinatally HIV-infected children: a concern for adolescence. J Int AIDS Soc. 2013 Jan;16(1):18603.
48. Moore DR. The diagnosis and management of auditory processing disorder. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2011;42(July):303–8.
Článek vyšel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 9
- S diagnostikou Parkinsonovy nemoci může nově pomoci AI nástroj pro hodnocení mrkacího reflexu
- Je libo čepici místo mozkového implantátu?
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Pomůže v budoucnu s triáží na pohotovostech umělá inteligence?
- AI může chirurgům poskytnout cenná data i zpětnou vazbu v reálném čase
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- Graviola (Annona muricata) attenuates behavioural alterations and testicular oxidative stress induced by streptozotocin in diabetic rats
- CH(II), a cerebroprotein hydrolysate, exhibits potential neuro-protective effect on Alzheimer’s disease
- Comparison between Aptima Assays (Hologic) and the Allplex STI Essential Assay (Seegene) for the diagnosis of Sexually transmitted infections
- Assessment of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase activity using CareStart G6PD rapid diagnostic test and associated genetic variants in Plasmodium vivax malaria endemic setting in Mauritania