Beta power encodes contextual estimates of temporal event probability in the human brain
Authors:
Alessandro Tavano aff001; Erich Schröger aff001; Sonja A. Kotz aff003
Authors place of work:
BioCog, Cognitive Incl. Biological Psychology, Institute of Psychology, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany
aff001; Department of Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute for Empirical Aesthetics, Frankfurt am Main, Germany
aff002; Department of Neuropsychology, Max-Planck-Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany
aff003; Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Department of Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Maastricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands
aff004
Published in the journal:
PLoS ONE 14(9)
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0222420
Summary
To prepare for an impending event of unknown temporal distribution, humans internally increase the perceived probability of event onset as time elapses. This effect is termed the hazard rate of events. We tested how the neural encoding of hazard rate changes by providing human participants with prior information on temporal event probability. We recorded behavioral and electroencephalographic (EEG) data while participants listened to continuously repeating five-tone sequences, composed of four standard tones followed by a non-target deviant tone, delivered at slow (1.6 Hz) or fast (4 Hz) rates. The task was to detect a rare target tone, which equiprobably appeared at either position two, three or four of the repeating sequence. In this design, potential target position acts as a proxy for elapsed time. For participants uninformed about the target’s distribution, elapsed time to uncertain target onset increased response speed, displaying a significant hazard rate effect at both slow and fast stimulus rates. However, only in fast sequences did prior information about the target’s temporal distribution interact with elapsed time, suppressing the hazard rate. Importantly, in the fast, uninformed condition pre-stimulus power synchronization in the beta band (Beta 1, 15–19 Hz) predicted the hazard rate of response times. Prior information suppressed pre-stimulus power synchronization in the same band, while still significantly predicting response times. We conclude that Beta 1 power does not simply encode the hazard rate, but—more generally—internal estimates of temporal event probability based upon contextual information.
Keywords:
Behavior – Probability distribution – Electroencephalography – Permutation – attention – Scalp – Probability density – Electrode potentials
Introduction
How do humans successfully prepare for future event onset? When uninformed about the event’s temporal distribution, attention mounts with elapsed time, concurrently increasing response speed and accuracy at event onset [1–17]. This time-dependent performance gain seems to depend on a time-dependent accrual in perceived event probability, termed the hazard rate of events. The hazard rate is thought to be endogenously generated by normalizing implicit estimates of target probability (probability density), by the ever-diminishing, time-dependent probability that the event has not yet occurred (survival probability) [12, 18].
Neural circuits are sensitive to the hazard rate. Shadlen and colleagues [10, 19] showed that neurons in the macaque Lateral Intraparietal Area (LIP) maintain a neocortical representation of the hazard rate. Firing rates in LIP neurons increase with elapsed time irrespective of whether a motor response is given ([19–20]; for a similar result in monkey prefrontal cortex, [21]). In humans, hazard rate effects on cortical activity have been found in regions homologous to the LIP area (Intraparietal Sulcus, IPS: [22–24]; Inferior Parietal Cortex, IPC, [3]), but also in motor, sensory, and prefrontal regions (Supplementary Motor Area, SMA, and the right Superior Temporal Gyrus, STG, for auditory stimuli, 8; premotor cortex, [25]), suggesting the existence of a distributed brain network encoding the hazard rate [5].
We asked how the hazard rate and its neural underpinnings change when participants are informed about the temporal distribution of an event. To this end, we collected human behavioral and electroencephalographic (EEG) data while participants detected an auditory target events which rarely occurred at different positions within a continuous repeating, five-tone sound sequence. Optimal performance required participants to cycle through potential target onset positions in preparing to respond, in case a target did eventually onset. Humans typically build temporal expectations for uncertain future events. For example, at any given time a soccer player may or may not receive the ball, but she still needs to update her probability estimates about when the ball will be kicked in order to be ready to catch it. The repeating sequence was composed of four standard tones (440 Hz) and a final higher-pitch one (494 Hz; for an overview of the experimental paradigm, see [26–27]). A rare low-pitch target tone (349 Hz) appeared only in 20% of the sequences, substituting a standard tone at either position two, three, or four, with equal probability (Fig 1A and Experimental design). In this design, elapsed time is represented by the within-sequence isochronous unfolding of potential event onset positions within each repeating tone sequence. We expected participants to update target probability estimates within each sequential cycle and response times to reflect a hazard rate distribution (Fig 1B). An increase in perceived temporal probability estimates should also boost early neural sensory processes for events that eventually occur [28–33] as well as late attention-capturing responses [34–39].
However, if prior information is provided to participants about target probability across positions within a sequence, the hazard rate should not be generated as participants would not need to build the survival probability to respond, as simply relying on prior information (equiprobable distribution) would suffice. Our experiment was organized into two sessions. In a first session, participants were uninformed about the target’s distribution. In a second session, participants were informed about the equiprobable target distribution within each repeating sequence. Importantly, in both sessions stimulus sequences were delivered using two different rhythms: first slow (1.66 Hz, resulting from a constant 750 ms Stimulus-Onset-Asynchrony, SOA), and then fast (4 Hz, 250 ms SOA). We hypothesized that fast sequences should favor the perception of five-tone sequences as an auditory object per se, making it easier for participants to apply prior information about target position probability.
Recent work suggests that activity in the beta band (13–30 Hz) is a likely candidate for the neural encoding of event probability estimates. Beta-band activity tracks the temporal regularity of isochronous tone sequences [40–42] and correlates with behavioral accuracy [43–45]. In purely perceptual tasks, beta-band power synchronizes [46] with the immediate onset a stimulus [40–41]. Engel and Fries [47] first framed a role for beta-band oscillations in endogenously registering the maintenance or change of a current internal state via cortical feedback projections [48–52]. While previous work analyzed power across the whole beta band, three independent lines of evidence point to a specific role of the low beta-band range, also termed Beta 1 rhythm (< 20 Hz) in perception: 1) Simulation work on neural activity across cortical layers suggests a role for Beta 1 oscillations in distinguishing between novel and standard events [51]; 2) Motor output inhibition has been more frequently linked to high beta-band or Beta 2 activity (> 20 Hz) [52]; 3) Low beta-band (~15 Hz) activity of basal ganglia origin appears to encode an event’s task relevance (target vs. non-target) regardless of motor output [53].
Results show that in fast sequences lower beta band power (Beta 1 rhythm, 15–19 Hz) encodes the hazard rate of events. Providing prior information on target probability suppressed Beta 1 activity as well as the hazard rate of response time, hence reflecting the discrete uniform distribution of targets. Crucially, suppressed Beta 1 power still predicted behavior, suggesting that it does not simply encode the hazard rate, but more generally context-specific temporal probability estimates.
Results
Twenty-six young adults participated in the experiment, which was organized according to a 2 x 2 design with factors prior information and Stimulus Onset Asynchrony (SOA). Prior information tested the effect of providing participants with explicit information about the distribution of target tones, while SOA tested whether the effects of presentation rate on extracting stimulus statistics [32–33]. In a first session (uninformed), participants were asked to detect the low pitch tone (target) with tones delivered first at a slow rate (constant SOA = 750 ms) and then at a fast rate (constant SOA = 250 ms). In a second session (informed), participants were explicitly informed about the repeating sequence structure and the target’s equal probability distribution across standard positions two, three and four. Each participant received four conditions in the following order: 1) slow uninformed; 2) fast uninformed; 3) slow informed; 4) fast informed.
The hazard rate of response times
Accuracy in target detection, as reflected by task sensitivity (d’) measures, was very high and did not differ among conditions (all Fs(1,25) ≤ 2.82, all ps ≥ 0.12; Fig 2A). To analyze response times, we fitted nonparametric Theil-Sen estimators [54–55] to single-trial response times across successive potential target positions, our predictor variable. The Theil-Sen estimator is the median of all the lines connecting two data points, and is more robust to outliers as compared to least-squares fit. There resulted one intercept and one regression coefficient (or slope) per participant/condition. Intercept estimates reflect changes in response speed magnitude unrelated to elapsed time, while regression coefficients specifically partial out the effects of elapsed time. The analysis of intercepts showed that participants were overall quicker at responding to targets appearing within faster rather than slower sequences (F(1,25) = 40.25, p < 0.001; Fig 2B), suggesting a generic facilitation effect for target processing in faster sequences, but we found no effect of prior information and no interaction (all Fs(1,25) ≤ 1.12, all ps ≥ 0.30). The average response speed for fast sequences was 348 ms (Standard Error of the Mean, SEM = 22), and 411 ms for slow sequences (SEM = 36).
Slopes convey time-dependent changes in response speed using a signed value: a negative value indexes the presence of the hazard rate, as response latencies decrease with elapsed time. Slopes distribution was normal in all conditions (all Shapiro-Wilks Statistics ≥ 0.84, all ps > 0.21). We found a significant prior information × SOA interaction (F(1,25) = 4.73, p < 0.05) driven by a significant effect of prior information in fast sequences (t(1,25) = -3.33, p < 0.01, all pairwise t-tests fdr-corrected, here and in the following): the hazard rate was present in the fast uninformed condition (t(1,25) = 0.05, p = 0.95), but not in the fast informed condition (t(1,25) = 0.05, p = 0.95). The hazard rate was present for both slow conditions regardless of information status (all ts(1,25) ≤ -6.06, all ps < 0.001). Overall, when significant, the hazard rate effect resulted in a speed gain between 10 and 15 ms per potential target position (Fig 2C).
The suppression of the hazard rate effect by prior information in fast sequences might also have been caused to the cancelling out of random behavioral patterns at the group level, rather than to individually consistent cognitive changes. To verify whether that was the case, we subtracted slope estimates obtained in the uninformed condition from those obtained in the informed condition at either SOA level, and tested the observed data against the null hypothesis that prior information was equally likely to suppress or enhance the hazard rate. A binomial test indicated that in fast sequences prior information suppressed the hazard rate in 20 out 26 participants, that is, in 77% of our sample (p < 0.01, Fig 2C), suggesting a non-random effect.
The hazard rate of sensory processing
First, we verified whether attention had been effectively captured by the five-tone sequence cycle. We hypothesized that if participants perceived non-target tones as the last of each sequence, their onset should have elicited a Mismatch Negativity response signaling pitch-change detection: this hypothesis was verified in all conditions (all ps < 0.05, see S1A Fig), suggesting that the non-target deviant effectively reset attention at the end of each five-tone sequence.
We then analyzed the components of the event-related potentials reflecting the elapsed-time component of target onset. A single-trial Theil-Sen regression was run for each time sample at electrode level (epoch duration: 750 ms, including 250-ms pre-stimulus time) and repeated for each participant and condition. There resulted event-related regressions coefficients (ERRCs), which encode the effect of elapsed time on event-related potentials as a difference wave across potential target position, and event-related intercepts (ERIs), which instead encode brain activity unrelated to the passing of time, on a pair with the behavioral analysis [56].
We ran a hypothesis-free, cluster permutation analysis [57] condition-wise to determine the presence of significant activation relative to a surrogate distribution obtained by randomizing data points along the time axis, and then analyzed the effect of prior information by contrasting informed and uninformed conditions within each SOA level. ERIs showed significant characteristic peaks of activity labelled as CNV (Contingent Negative Variation; [25]), N1, N2b, and P3 in all conditions (all ps < 0.001). Stimulus rate and prior information resulted in larger N2b/P3 deflections for slower vs. faster sequences, and uninformed vs. informed conditions, respectively (all ps < 0.01, S1B Fig; [35]). This confirms previous findings that increasing perceptual expectations reduces surprise-related attentive sound processing.
As for ERRCs, we first looked at the fast, uninformed condition. We found significant N1 and P3 deflections, with a characteristic topography [58] (p < 0.001; Fig 3A, upper panel), and a significant late negativity cluster in the informed condition (> 410 ms post onset, p = 0.05, not shown). When testing the effects of prior information, two significant clusters emerged again at N1 and P3 latencies (Fig 3A, lower panel).
We found a larger N1 deflection (cluster latency 58–156 ms, p = 0.018), and a larger P3 deflection in the uninformed condition (cluster latency 257–322 ms, p = 0.016). Notably, N1 amplitude significantly predicted response time slopes regardless of information status (uninformed, rho = 0.51, p < 0.01; informed, rho = 0.61, p = 0.001, Steiger’s Z = -0.52, p > 0.60, Fig 3B, upper panel), while P3 amplitudes predicted behavior only in the informed condition (uninformed, rho = 0.01, p = 0.99; informed, rho = 0.43, p < 0.05, Fig 3B, lower panel). A topographical analysis (TANOVA, [59]) showed that prior information attenuated P3 activity without changing the underlying generator configuration, while N1 suppression likely depended on changing neural generator between 55 and 100 ms post target onset (S1C Fig). We conclude that for fast sequences, elapsed time enhanced neural afferent activity (N1 wave, [60]) for uninformed participants, thereby increasing response speed. Factoring out elapsed time via prior information correlatively reduced sensory processing and response speed.
ERRCs in slow sequences showed significant brain activity in the P3 range regardless of information status (all ps < 0.001), while the N1 deflection was significant only in the uninformed condition (p < 0.05, informed: p = 0.28; see Fig 3C). However, we found no significant prior information effect (p = 0.23). P3 ERRCs significantly predicted response time slopes regardless of information status (all rhos > 0.48, ps < 0.01). Thus, EEG and behavioral data concur to suggest that the down effects of prior information are more likely to be detected using faster stimulus rates [61].
The hazard rate of neural power
We then focused the analysis of EEG power in sequences. The Theil-Sen estimator was applied to each time-frequency bin, obtaining a distribution of signed time-frequency regression coefficients (TFRCs), measured as μV2 unit change per potential target position with negative/positive sign equaling time-dependent decrease/increase. A rank correlation analysis was run between each TFRC bin and response time slopes for the entire epoch (from 250 ms pre-stimulus to 500 ms post-stimulus), in order to isolate the task-relevant components of spectral power.
In the following, we focus on fast sequences in order to highlight the effect prior information. For participants who were uninformed about the target’s temporal distribution, we expected single-trial power in the Beta 1 range (< 20 Hz) to increase with elapsed time, encoding the task relevance of a potential target onset. We measured the power spectrum of each trial epoch, from 5 to 28 Hz. A concentration in Beta 1 power (15–19 Hz, averaged across all scalp electrodes) at a latency of about -150 ms (pre-stimulus baseline), inversely correlated with response time slopes (p < 0.05, Fig 4A, left panel). Notice that, by design, participants could not foresee whether a target would appear or not at any potential position within the current sequence. This suggests that synchrony in the beta band encodes the update of time-dependent probability estimates for potential future events, driving an increase in response speed for targets which eventually did appear. Beta 1 power synchrony was significant at left centroparietal electrodes (p < 0.05, Fig 4A, right panel). Importantly, Beta 1 power also predicted post-stimulus ERRC N1 amplitudes (p = 0.01), establishing for the first time a direct connection between pre-stimulus probability update and post-stimulus sensory processing.
With prior information, probability update processes in the Beta 1 band were suppressed at central electrodes, and a significant decrease in spectral power was found at parietal electrodes (p < 0.05, Fig 4A, right panel). This may reflect how the brain silences the hazard rate at sensory-specific central electrodes. Notably, the correlation of Beta 1 power with response time slopes remained significant (p < 0.05, average across all scalp electrodes, Fig 4A, left panel). Similarly, Beta 1 power maintained a significant correlation with post-stimulus N1 amplitudes (p < 0.05), confirming the inference of a causal link between pre-stimulus Beta 1 power and sensory processing. We found no significant correlation with response times for either Alpha (8–12 Hz, all ps > 0.2) or Beta 2 (21–25 Hz, all ps > 0.08) bands, suggesting the encoding of perceived event probability was specific to Beta 1 rhythm.
Discussion
To prepare for future events, humans could simply estimate the probability density function of target onset over trials and use that information to respond [62, 63]. However, “that is not the natural way one thinks about it as the [waiting] process unfolds in time. Rather, if the event has not yet occurred, one senses there is some tendency for it to occur the next instant in time” [10,12,19]. We found that such a “feeling” is present also for target events that may or may not onset within a given interval. Participants naïve to target distribution probabilities used elapsed time to progressively increase their temporal expectations about potential target onset, regardless of whether stimulus rate was slow or fast. However, when informed about the equiprobable potential target onset within each five-tone sequence, a suppression effect of prior information on the hazard rate was detected in fast sequences only. We ascribe the absence of a prior information effect in slow sequences to a difficulty for participants to represent five-tone sequences as an integrated percept within sensory memory [26–27, 64–66]. Notice that the attenuation of the hazard rate in fast sequences was not due to increased variance at group level but rather to suppressive processes acting at individual level (in ~ 77% of our sample). We suggest that prior information factored out elapsed-time dependent processes, such as the calculation of survival probability, from the computation of position-wise target probability. However, it is important to underlie that the discreet uniform probability density, which is assumed to be endogenously computed by participants, coincides with the distribution highlighted by prior information, so at present their contribution to the effect prior information on response times in fast sequences indistinguishable.
We found an increase in early post-stimulus sensory activity in the N1 range in the uninformed condition in fast sequences. This increase in brain activity explained a significant proportion of individual level variance in response speed, suggesting that modulations of N1 coefficients encode time-dependent information. However, prior information significantly suppressed the N1 deflection, while remarkably maintaining a consistent predictive relationship to response behavior, suggesting that encoding temporal probability changes the weights of sensory processing.
Low beta-band power (Beta 1 = 15–19 Hz), ~150–125 ms before the potential onset of the target event, consistently predicted the effect of elapsed time on response speed and sensory processing in both uninformed and informed fast sequence conditions. For uninformed participants, with elapsed time factored in, Beta 1 increased at central electrodes. When elapsed time was factored out by prior information, Beta 1 did not change at central electrodes and was significant suppressed at parietal electrodes. The latter result could reflect how a distributed neural system silences the sensory effects of elapsed time.
The finding of a Beta 1 power modulation as a neural correlate of the hazard rate expands on the original proposal of Engel and Fries [50], as well as the findings of Arnal et al. [43] and Fujioka et al. [40–41], by demonstrating that beta power does not simply reflect temporal tracking of event onset, but rather subjective estimates of temporal probability. Todorovic and colleagues [67] found beta-band desynchronization for expected auditory events, but only in a passive listening condition, with no difference when attention was deployed on sound sequences. Spitzer and Haegens [50] suggest a primary role of beta band in the endogenous re-activation of neuronal ensembles coding for task-relevant information (see also [48–49, 51]). A potentially fruitful endeavor for future research would be to bridge human and animal research by specifically testing the relationship between cortical Beta 1 power and the encoding of the hazard rate at the neuronal level [10, 19].
The neural mechanisms underlying elapsed time to target under most distribution functions (except exponential ones, also termed memoryless functions, for which no hazard rate is elicited [12]) constitute a particular case of perceptual bias. Participants gain a substantial behavioral advantage by constructing subjective temporal expectations rather than relying on probability density estimates. We showed that providing truthful prior information on the target’s uniform probability density cancels any behavioral advantage in fast sequences. It follows that the hazard rate of events represents a conceptual challenge for active sensing models of perception that construe the fit between brain and behavior as based on precise internal models of actual event statistics, which invariably ascribe a facilitative effect to veridical prior information [28].
Contrary to previous work with deterministic targets within a foreperiod design [68–69], we did not find significant effects of elapsed time in the alpha band. This may be due to the sequential cycling of attention, which may have entrained sub-bands of the alpha rhythm or to the fact that we used an uncertain target. It is also possible that the use of target sounds may have accentuated the relevance of low beta relative to alpha activity [70].
It has been postulated that changes in neural rhythms may underlie different cognitive operations [71]. Notably, simulations studies of cortical rhythm formation postulate that the Beta 1 rhythm reflects memory of stimulation history [51]. The importance of cortical Beta 1 in defining the task-relevance of sensory stimuli could depends on contributions from basal ganglia generator circuits [53]. Interestingly, recent clinical work purports that beta desynchronization in Parkinson’s disease (PD), predominantly of basal ganglia origin, specifically impairs pre-stimulus beta-band activity in rhythmic auditory perception, suggesting a causal relationship with timing deficits that are typically present in PD [72]. This patient group could provide a test case to better analyze the functional specificity of synchronization and desynchronization in the low vs. high beta band, contrasting movement initiation and task relevance [46].
Conclusions
Modulations of Beta 1 rhythm reflect how participants internally prepare to respond to uncertain events, keeping into account what they know about their temporal distribution.
Materials and methods
Participants
The experiment was conducted at the Max Planck Institute for Human and Cognitive Brain Sciences, Leipzig (Germany). Thirty healthy young adult individuals (15 female; age range = 19–31, mean = 25, SD = 3.5) were recruited from the institute’s database of participants. All individuals had university-level education and were paid for their participation. Four participants were excluded from further analysis: two for below-average behavioral performance (less than 50% target detection), one for misinterpreting task instructions, one for excessive number of rejected target epochs (cut-off: 80%, i.e. at least 16 target trials per position and condition). The final pool of 26 participants (13 females) reported no neurological or psychiatric disorders or therapies involving the central nervous system. Individually measured, bilateral audiometric thresholds of at least 30 dB Hearing Level at 0.25–8 KHz octave frequencies [73]. All participants signed a written informed consent complying with the Declaration of Helsinki on human experimentation. This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Leipzig.
Stimuli
Stimuli were three 50-ms pure tones (5 ms rise/fall), binaurally presented via loudspeakers at ~80 dB SPL and generated using Matlab (version 7, Mathworks, Natick, MA). A 440 Hz tone (A4 on the equal tempered scale), termed standard tone, was presented 900 times per condition (75% global probability). A 494 Hz tone (B4, two semitones higher than the standard), termed non-target tone, was presented 240 times per condition (20% global probability). A 349 Hz tone (F4, four semitones lower than the standard), termed target tone, was presented 60 times per condition (5% global probability). Fig 1 illustrates stimulus organization. Standard and non-target tones were organized as a continuously repeating sequence of four standard tones followed by a non-target tone always in fifth position. Target tones appeared in 20% of the sequences, randomly at either standard position two, three or four, with a uniform distribution. There was a maximum of one target per sequence, and minimally two successive sequences without targets before a target-containing sequence. Stimulus sequences were delivered using Presentation software (version 12.0, www.neurobs.com) running on a Windows PC.
Experimental design
Participants sat in an electrically shielded, sound-attenuated chamber, and fixated a white cross on black computer screen at a distance of ~1 meter while listening to the auditory stimuli. They responded to target tone onset by pressing a button on an external response box.
Targets were equiprobably distributed across standard position two, three and four, yielding a discrete uniform distribution function: f(t) = 1/3, for each of standard positions two, three, and four. Denoting the survival probability (“the event has not yet occurred”) as 1 − F(t), where F(t) is the cumulative distribution function, the hazard function is then: h(t) = f(t)/(1-F(t)).
The experiment was organized into two successive sessions, with a fixed order. In the first session, participants were unaware of target distribution and instructed to respond to the onset of target tones as accurately and fast as possible by pressing a button on a response box. They trained in a short block of 60 experimental randomly distributed tone sequences containing three targets. If errors were made (Missing, False Alarm), the training block was repeated until no errors were detected. Experimental tone sequences were first delivered with a constant 750-ms stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA), corresponding to 1.6 Hz stimulus rate (three 5-min blocks; first condition), and then—after a short break—with a constant 250-ms SOA, corresponding to 4 Hz stimulus rate (one 5-min block; second condition). The first two conditions were tagged as slow uninformed and fast uninformed, respectively. In a second session, after a 15-min break, participants were informed, both verbally and using visual aids, both about the repeating tone sequence and target probability distribution within a sequence. The instruction, therefore, changed: they were asked to respond to target tones whose onset would break the sequence at either standard position two, three, or four. They again trained with a short block of 60 tones and 3 targets. If errors were made (Missing, False Alarm), the training block was repeated until no errors were detected. The experimental tone trains were again first delivered with a 750-ms constant SOA (three 5-min blocks; third condition), and then—after a short break—with a 250-ms constant SOA (one 5-min block; fourth condition). These conditions were tagged as slow informed and fast informed, respectively. Participants completed the experiment in about one hour.
Behavioral data analysis
Hits were all button presses whose response time ranged between 150 and 1250 ms from the onset of a target tone. Button presses recorded after 1250 ms were considered as false alarms (FA). Accuracy was measured by z-transforming hit and false alarm counts (5% adjustment for ceiling effects) to obtain a d’ index of task sensitivity (zFA—zHit, [74]). A single-trial linear fit allowed detecting the presence/absence of the hazard rate: slope sign indicates an increase or decrease in response times for long awaited events, while slope magnitude measures the strength of change with elapsed time. The Theil-Sen estimator represents an unbiased, robust, non-parametric simple linear regression method, which extracts the median slope among all possible pairwise combinations of points (54–55). Accuracy and reaction time data entered a two-way, repeated-measures ANOVA, with the factors SOA (slow, fast) and prior information (uninformed, informed). Results with p ≤ 0.05 (fdr-correction was applied to protect against family-wise error rate) were declared significant.
EEG data acquisition and pre-processing
Electroencephalographic (EEG) data were collected using a 26 scalp Ag/AgCl electrode set (BrainAmp), mounted in an elastic cap according to the 10–20 International system. The electrode space was composed by: Fp1, Fpz, Fp2, F7, F3, Fz, F4, F8, FT7, FC3, FC4, FT8, T7, C3, Cz, C4, T8, CP5, CP6, P7, P3, Pz, P4, P8, O1, O2. Two external electrodes were placed at right and left mastoid sites. For electrooculographic (EOG) data recording, four additional electrodes were placed at both eye canthi, and above and below the right eye. For participants 19 to 26 (21–30 in the original dataset), the cap contained 38 more electrodes (10–10 system), not used in the current analysis for comparability across participants (Fpz was excluded from participants 1–18 as it was conversely not present in participants 19–26). An online reference was placed on the tip of the nose and the sternum served as ground. Electrode impedance was kept below 5 kΩ. EEG/EOG sampling rate was set to 500 Hz, with online high-pass filtering at 0.01 Hz. The resulting continuous recordings were visually inspected and pruned from non-stereotypical artefacts or extreme voltage changes values. An Independent Component Analysis (ICA, extended Infomax, [75]) was performed on the pruned continuous data, offline bandpass filtered 1–100 Hz (Kaiser window, Beta 5.6533, filter order 1812 points, transition bandwidth 1 Hz, see [76]). The maps of exemplar Independent Components (ICs) reflecting blinks or vertical eye movements and horizontal eye movements from one participant were selected as spatial templates in a semi-automatic artefact search across all ICs of the remaining datasets (correlation threshold, r = 0.94; [77]). Eye-movement-related ICs, both vertical/blink-related and horizontal, ranged between 1 and 3 per participant. Selected ICs were verified in their spectral power distribution before being subtracted [78]. The resulting continuous datasets were low-pass filtered at 35 Hz (filter order 184, transition bandwidth 10 Hz).
Event-related potential and coefficient analysis
Epochs were separately extracted for the onset of standard, non-target and target stimuli, beginning 1000 ms before and ending 1000 ms after stimulus onset. Epochs were selected based on their contribution to increasing the signal-to-noise ratio [79]. On average, 12.3% of epochs were rejected. Epochs of interest for regression analysis began 250 ms before the onset of target tone, and ended 500 ms thereafter. Neural responses for non-target tones were calculated in sequences that did not contain a target trial, by subtracting the response of the fourth standard tone. Stimulation was isochronous, so that target position directly reflects elapsed time. For each time point of each Target trial, a Theil-Sen estimate of the linear relationship between target position as predictor and event-related electrical activity was calculated. There resulted event-related regression coefficients (ERRCs, slopes) and event-related intercepts (ERIs). ERRCs encode neural estimates of elapsed time [56, 80]. We took a data-driven approach to analyze the distribution and polarity of neural effects within a post-stimulus window of interest (0–600 ms). The presence of differences in the effects of Target position (elapsed time) as determined by prior information was tested for all channels/time points separately at each SOA level: slow uninformed vs. slow informed; fast uninformed vs. fast informed. ERRCs entered a non-parametric, cluster-based permutation test of significance, testing the effect of prior information at each SOA level [57]. Clusters were minimally composed of two electrodes. Cluster-level statistics was determined by summating significant paired T-test values (cluster alpha = 0.05) across adjacent points within each cluster, and evaluated under the distribution obtained by drawing 1000 within-subject, random permutations of the observed data. Cluster-based tests against zero were run by contrasting each condition with a surrogate distribution obtained by randomizing individual data points along the time axis. Significance was set at p = 0.05. Topographical differences in the distribution of current density were investigated using the Global Dissimilarity Index, which measures the configuration of electric fields (and their linear transformations), normalized by their individual strength [59].
Time-frequency analysis
To increase signal-to noise ratio, data were subject to a Principal Component Analysis, and the first four components, accounting on average for 93% of variance, were retained for further processing. Zero-padded (5 s), individual target epochs were submitted to a time-frequency analysis at each electrode using a Morlet wavelet (7 cycles, [81]), for frequencies comprised between 5 and 28 Hz, in steps of 0.25 Hz. Event-related power estimates of target position effects at each frequency/time point were extracted from 500 ms pre-stimulus to 500 ms post-stimulus (sliding window = 25 ms). Each time-frequency bin entered a Theil-Sen, non-parametric regression analysis with Target position as predictor, obtaining time-frequency regression coefficients (TFRCs), and time-frequency intercepts (TFIs). We obtained median TFRC power estimates across all electrodes at Alpha (8–12 Hz), Beta 1 (14–19 Hz) and Beta 2 (20–25 Hz) rhythms, and calculated a Kendall-type [82] rank correlation between each TFRC and response time slopes in order to determine which time-frequency band were relevant for behavior. A permutation resampling approach (1000 repetitions) was used to test the significance of rank correlation results.
To analyze the effect of prior information within each SOA level, we again resorted to a cluster-based permutation approach of power estimates between 250 ms pre-stimulus and 500 ms post-stimulus, using paired T-tests (significance set at 0.05, minimal N electrodes per cluster = 1, T-test cluster parameter = maxsize, 1000 permutations, cluster alpha set at 0.1; [57]). All analyses were run using EEGLAB [75], FieldTrip [81], and custom Matlab scripts. The datasets generated during and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request for researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data of the Max Planck Institute for Human Brain Sciences.
Supporting information
S1 Fig [eps]
Attention cycles, event-related intercepts (ERI) and N1-P3 event-related coefficients (ERRCs).
Zdroje
1. Bermudez MA, Schultz W. Timing in reward and decision processes. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014; 369: 20120468. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0468 24446502
2. Bertelson P. The time course of preparation. Q J Exp Psychol. 1967; 19: 272–279. doi: 10.1080/14640746708400102 6074169
3. Bolger D, Coull JT, Schön D. Metrical rhythm implicitly orients attention in time as indexed by improved target detection and left inferior parietal activation. J Cogn Neurosci. 2014; 26: 593–605. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00511 24168222
4. Correa Á, Lupiáñez J, Milliken P, Tudela P. Endogenous temporal orienting of attention in detection and discrimination tasks. Percept Psychophys. 2004; 66: 264–278. doi: 10.3758/bf03194878 15129748
5. Correa Á, Nobre AC. Neural modulation by regularity and passage of time. J Neurophysiol. 2008; 100: 1649–1655. doi: 10.1152/jn.90656.2008 18632896
6. Coull JT, Cheng RK, Meck WH. Neuroanatomical and neurochemical substrates of timing. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011; 36: 3–25.
7. Coull JT, Nobre AC. Dissociating explicit timing from temporal expectations with fMRI. Curr Op Neurobiol. 2008; 18: 137–144. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.011 18692573
8. Cui X, Stetson C, Montague PR, Eagleman DM. Ready … Go: Amplitude of the fMRI signal encodes expectation of cue arrival time. Plos Biol. 2009; 7: e1000167. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1000167 19652698
9. Griffin IC, Miniussi C, Nobre AC. Orienting attention in time. Front Biosci. 2001; 6: D660–671. doi: 10.2741/griffin 11282565
10. Janssen P, Shadlen MN. A neural representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP. Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8: 234–241. doi: 10.1038/nn1386 15657597
11. Jones MR, Moynihan H, MacKenzie N, Puente J. Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychol Sci. 2002; 13: 313–319. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00458 12137133
12. Luce RD. Response times: their role in inferring elementary mental organization. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1986.
13. Nobre K, Coull JT. Attention and time. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2010.
14. Novak GP, Ritter W, Vaughan JR, Wiznitzer ML. Differentiation of negative event related potentials in an auditory discrimination task. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1990; 75: 255–275. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(90)90105-s 1691075
15. Niemi P, Näätänen R. Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychol Bull. 1981: 89; 133–162.
16. Vangkilde S, Coull JT, Bundesen C. Great expectations: Temporal expectation modulates perceptual processing speed. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 2012; 38: 1183–1119. doi: 10.1037/a0026343 22250866
17. Woodrow H. The measurement of attention. Psychol Monogr. 1914;17: 5.
18. Elithorn A, Lawrence C. Central inhibition: some refractory observations. Q J Exp Psychol. 1955; 11: 211–220.
19. Yang T, Shadlen MN. Probabilistic reasoning by neurons. Nature 2007; 447: 1075–1080. doi: 10.1038/nature05852 17546027
20. Maimon G, Assad JA. A cognitive signal for the proactive timing of action in macaque LIP. Nat Neurosci. 2006; 9: 948–955. doi: 10.1038/nn1716 16751764
21. Genovesio A, Tsujimoto S, Wise SP. Neuronal activity related to elapsed time in prefrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2006; 95: 3281–3285. doi: 10.1152/jn.01011.2005 16421197
22. Cotti J, Rohenkohl G, Stokes M, Nobre AC, Coull JT. Functionally dissociating temporal and motor components of response preparation in left intraparietal sulcus. Neuroimage 2011; 54: 1221–1230. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.038 20868756
23. Coull JT, Cotti J, Vidal F. Increasing activity in Left Inferior Parietal Cortex and right Prefrontal Cortex with increasing temporal predictability: An fMRI study of the hazard rate. Proc Soc Behav Sci. 2014; 126: 41–44.
24. Davranche K, Nazarian B, Vidal F, Coull JT. Orienting attention in time activates left intraparietal sulcus for perceptual and motor task goals. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011; 23: 3318–3330. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00030 21452942
25. Hultin L, Rossini P, Romani GL, Högstedt P, Tecchio F, Pizzella V. Neuromagnetic localization of the late component of the contingent negative variation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1996; 98: 435–448. doi: 10.1016/0013-4694(96)95507-8 8763503
26. Sussman E, Winkler I, Huotilainen M, Ritter W, Näätänen R. Top-down effects can modify the initially stimulus-driven auditory organization. Cog Brain Res. 2002; 13: 393–405.
27. Sussman E, Gumenyuk V. Organization of sequential sounds in auditory memory. NeuroReport 2005; 16: 1519–1523. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000177002.35193.4c 16110282
28. Friston K. A theory of cortical responses. Philos Trans R Soc London Biol Sci. 2005; 360: 815–836.
29. Garrido MI, Kilner JM, Kiebel SJ, Friston KJ. Dynamic causal modeling of the response to frequency deviants. J Neurophysiol. 2009a; 101: 2620–2631.
30. Garrido MI, Kilner JM, Stephan KE, Friston KJ. The mismatch negativity: a review of underlying mechanisms. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009b; 120: 453–463.
31. Jaramillo S, Zador AM. The auditory cortex mediates the perceptual effects of acoustic temporal expectation. Nat Neurosci. 2011; 14: 246–251. doi: 10.1038/nn.2688 21170056
32. Tavano A, Widmann A, Bendixen A, Schröger E. Temporal regularity facilitates higher-order sensory predictions in fast auditory sequences. Eur J Neurosci. 2014; 39: 308–318. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12404 24236753
33. Wacongne C, Changeux JP, Dehaene S. A neuronal model of predictive coding accounting for the mismatch negativity. J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 3665–3678. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5003-11.2012 22423089
34. Ahveninen J, Kähkönen S, Pennanen S, Liesivuori J, Ilmoniemi RJ, Jääskeläinen IP. Tryptophan depletion effects on EEG and MEG responses suggest serotonergic modulation of auditory involuntary attention in humans. Neuroimage 2002; 16: 1052–1061. 12202092
35. Chennu S, Noreika V, Gueorguiev D, Blenkmann A, Kochen S, Ibáñez A, Owen AM, Bekinschtein TA. Expectation and Attention in Hierarchical Auditory Prediction. J Neurosci. 2013; 33: 11194–11205. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0114-13.2013 23825422
36. Miniussi C, Wilding EL, Coull JT, Nobre AC. Orienting attention in time: modulation of brain potentials. Brain 1999; 122: 1507–1518. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.8.1507 10430834
37. Schröger E, Marzecová A, SanMiguel I. Attention and prediction in human audition: a lesson from cognitive psychophysiology. Eur J Neurosci. 2015; 41: 641–664. doi: 10.1111/ejn.12816 25728182
38. Sokolov EN, Spinks JA, Näätänen R, Lyytinen H. Auditory event-related potentials in the study of the orienting response. In: Sokolov EN, Spinks JA, Näätänen R, Lyytinen H, editors. The orienting response in information processing. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2002. pp. 241–325.
39. Sussman ES. A new view on the MMN and attention debate: The role of context in processing auditory events. J Psychophysiol. 2007; 21: 164–175.
40. Fujioka T, Ross B, Trainor LJ. Beta-Band Oscillations Represent Auditory Beat and Its Metrical Hierarchy in Perception and Imagery. J Neurosci. 2015; 35: 15187–15198. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2397-15.2015 26558788
41. Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Large EW, Ross B. Internalized timing of isochronous sounds is represented in neuromagnetic β oscillations. J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 1791–1802. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4107-11.2012 22302818
42. Merchant H, Grahn J, Trainor LJ, Rohrmeier M, Fitch WT. Finding the beat: a neural perspective across humans and non-human primates. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2015; 370: 20140093. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2014.0093 25646516
43. Arnal LH, Doelling KB, Poeppel D. Delta–beta coupled oscillations underlie temporal prediction accuracy. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25: 3077–3085. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu103 24846147
44. Arnal LH, Giraud AL. Cortical oscillations and sensory predictions. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16: 390–398. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.05.003 22682813
45. Patel AD, Iversen JR. The evolutionary neuroscience of musical beat perception: The Action Simulation for Auditory Prediction (ASAP) hypothesis. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014; 8: 57. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00057 24860439
46. Pfurtscheller G, Graimann B, Huggins JE, Levine SP, Schuh LA. Spatiotemporal patterns of beta desynchronization and gamma synchronization in corticographic data during self-paced movement. Clin Neurophysiol. 2003; 114: 1226–1236. 12842719
47. Engel AK, Fries P. Beta-band oscillations—signalling the status quo? Curr Op Neurobiol. 2010; 20: 156–165. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.02.015 20359884
48. Michalareas G, Vezoli J, vam Pelt S, Schoffelen JM, Kennedy H, Fries P. Alpha-Beta and Gamma rhythms subserve feedback and feedforward influences among human visual cortical areas. Neuron 2016; 89: 384–397. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.12.018 26777277
49. Lee JH, Whittington MA, Kopell NJ. Top-down beta rhythms support selective attention via interlaminar interaction: a model. PLoS Comput Biol. 2013; 9: e1003164–1003123. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003164 23950699
50. Spitzer B, Haegens S. Beyond the Status Quo: A Role for Beta Oscillations in Endogenous Content (Re)Activation. eNeuro 2017; 4: ENEURO.0170-17.2017.
51. Kopell N, Whittington MA, Kramer MA. Neuronal assembly dynamics in the beta1 frequency range permits short-term memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2011; 108: 3779–3784. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1019676108 21321198
52. Brovelli A, Ding M, Ledberg A, Chen Y, Nakamura R, Bressler SL. Beta oscillations in a large-scale sensorimotor cortical network: directional influences revealed by Granger causality. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2004; 101: 9849–9854. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0308538101 15210971
53. Leventhal DK, Gage GJ, Schmidt R, Pettibone JR, Case AC, Berke JD. Basal Ganglia Beta oscillations accompany cue utilization. Neuron 2012; 73: 523–536. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.032 22325204
54. Theil H. A rank-invariant method of linear and polynomial regression analysis. I, II, III. Nederl Akad Wetensch Proc. 1950; 53: 386–392, 521–525, 1397–1412.
55. Sen PK. Estimates of the regression coefficient based on Kendall’s tau. J Am Statistical Assoc. 1968; 63: 1379–1389.
56. Hauk O, Davis MH, Ford M, Pulvermüller F, Marslen-Wilson WD. The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of ERP data. Neuroimage 2006; 30: 1383–1400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.048 16460964
57. Maris E, Oostenveld R. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. J Neurosci Methods 2007; 164: 177–190. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2007.03.024 17517438
58. Näätänen R, Picton T. The N1 wave of the human electric and magnetic response to sound: A review and an analysis of the component structure. Psychophysiol. 1987; 24: 375–425.
59. Murray MM, Brunet D, Michel CM. Topographic ERP analyses: a step-by-step tutorial review. Brain Topogr. 2008; 20: 249–264. doi: 10.1007/s10548-008-0054-5 18347966
60. Budd TW, Barry RJ, Gordon E, Rennie C, Michie PT. Decrement of the N1 auditory event-related potential with stimulus repetition: habituation vs. refractoriness. Int J Psychophysiol. 1998; 31: 51–68. 9934621
61. Bendixen A, Prinz W, Horvath J, Trujillo-Barreto NJ, Schröger E. Rapid extraction of auditory feature contingencies. Neuroimage 2008; 41: 1111–1119. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.040 18467129
62. Leon MI, Shadlen MN. Representation of time by neurons in the posterior parietal cortex of the macaque. Neuron 2003; 38: 317–327. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00185-5 12718864
63. Luft CD, Meeson A, Welchman AE, Kourtzi Z. Decoding the future from past experience: learning shapes predictions in early visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2015; 113: 3159–3171. doi: 10.1152/jn.00753.2014 25744884
64. Cowan N. What are the differences between long-term, short-term, and working memory? In: Lacaille J-C, Castellucci VF, Belleville S, Sossin WS, editors. Progress in Brain Research: Essence of Memory. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2008. pp. 323–338.
65. Cowan NJ, Saults S, Nugent LD. The role of absolute and relative amounts of time in forgetting within immediate memory: the case of tone-pitch comparisons. Psychon Bull Rev 1999; 4: 393–397.
66. Schulze K, Tillmann B. Working memory for pitch, timbre, and words. Memory 2015; 21: 377–395.
67. Todorovic A., Schoffelen J.-M., van Ede F., Maris E., de Lange FP. Temporal Expectation and Attention Jointly Modulate Auditory Oscillatory Activity in the Beta Band. PLoS ONE 2015; 10: e0120288. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120288 25799572
68. Rohenkohl G, Nobre AC. Alpha oscillations related to anticipatory attention follow temporal expectations. J Neurosci. 2011; 31: 14076–14084. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3387-11.2011 21976492
69. Wilsch A, Henry MJ, Herrmann B, Maess B, Obleser J. Alpha oscillatory dynamics index temporal expectation benefits in working memory. Cereb Cortex 2015; 25: 1938–1946. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu004 24488943
70. Chang A, Bosnyak DJ, Trainor LJ. Unpredicted Pitch Modulates Beta Oscillatory Power during Rhythmic Entrainment to a Tone Sequence. Front Psychol. 2016; 7: 327. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00327 27014138
71. Kopell N, Kramer MA, Malerba P, Whittington MA. Are different rhythms good for different functions? Front Human Neurosci. 2010; 4: 187.
72. Gulberti A, Moll CK, Hamel W, Buhmann C, Koeppen JA, Boelmans K, Zittel S, Gerloff C, Westphasl M, Schneider TR, Engel AK. Predictive timing functions of cortical beta oscillations are impaired in Parkinson’s disease and influenced by L-DOPA and deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic nucleus. Impaired beta-band timing functions in PD. Neuroimage: Clinical 2015; 9: 436–449.
73. American National Standards Institute. Specifications for audiometers (ANSI S3.6–1996). New York; 1996.
74. Macmillan NA, Creelman CD. Detection theory: A user’s guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1991.
75. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics. J Neurosci Methods 2004; 134: 9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 15102499
76. Widmann A, Schröger E, Maess B. Digital filter design for electrophysiological data—a practical approach. J Neurosci Methods 2015; 250: 34–46. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.08.002 25128257
77. Viola FC, Thorne J, Edmonds B, Schneider T, Eichele T, Debener S. Semi-automatic identification of independent components representing EEG artefact. Clin Neurophysiol. 2009; 120: 868–877. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.01.015 19345611
78. Onton J, Makeig S. Information-based modelling of event-related brain dynamics. Prog Brain Res. 2006; 159: 99–120. doi: 10.1016/S0079-6123(06)59007-7 17071226
79. Rahne T, von Specht H, Mühler R. Sorted averaging. Application to auditory event-related responses. J Neurosci Methods 2008; 172: 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2008.04.006 18499265
80. Dien J, Frishkoff GA, Cerbone A, Tucker DM. Parametric analysis of event-related potentials in semantic comprehension: evidence for parallel brain mechanisms. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2003; 15: 137–153. 12429366
81. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: Open Source Software for Advanced Analysis of MEG, EEG, and Invasive Electrophysiological Data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011; Article ID 156869: doi: 10.1155/2011/156869 21253357
82. Kendall M. A New Measure of Rank Correlation. Biometrika 1938; 30: 81–89.
Článek vyšel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 9
- S diagnostikou Parkinsonovy nemoci může nově pomoci AI nástroj pro hodnocení mrkacího reflexu
- Je libo čepici místo mozkového implantátu?
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Pomůže v budoucnu s triáží na pohotovostech umělá inteligence?
- AI může chirurgům poskytnout cenná data i zpětnou vazbu v reálném čase
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- Graviola (Annona muricata) attenuates behavioural alterations and testicular oxidative stress induced by streptozotocin in diabetic rats
- CH(II), a cerebroprotein hydrolysate, exhibits potential neuro-protective effect on Alzheimer’s disease
- Comparison between Aptima Assays (Hologic) and the Allplex STI Essential Assay (Seegene) for the diagnosis of Sexually transmitted infections
- Assessment of glucose-6-phosphate dehydrogenase activity using CareStart G6PD rapid diagnostic test and associated genetic variants in Plasmodium vivax malaria endemic setting in Mauritania