#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Treatment of children under 4 years of age with medulloblastoma and ependymoma in the HIT2000/HIT-REZ 2005 trials: Neuropsychological outcome 5 years after treatment


Authors: Holger Ottensmeier aff001;  Paul G. Schlegel aff001;  Matthias Eyrich aff001;  Johannes E. Wolff aff003;  Björn-Ole Juhnke aff004;  Katja von Hoff aff004;  Stefanie Frahsek aff001;  Rene Schmidt aff005;  Andreas Faldum aff005;  Gudrun Fleischhack aff006;  Andre von Bueren aff007;  Carsten Friedrich aff008;  Anika Resch aff008;  Monika Warmuth-Metz aff009;  Jürgen Krauss aff010;  Rolf D. Kortmann aff011;  Udo Bode aff012;  Joachim Kühl aff001;  Stefan Rutkowski aff004
Authors place of work: Department of Paediatric Haematology and Oncology, University Children's Hospital, University Medical Center, Wuerzburg, Germany aff001;  Comprehensive Cancer Center Mainfranken, University Medical Center, Wuerzburg, Germany aff002;  AbbvVie, Oncology Development, Chicago, Illinois, United States of America aff003;  Department of Paediatric Haematology and Oncology, University Medical Centre, Hamburg-Eppendorf, Hamburg, Germany aff004;  Institute of Biostatistics and Clinical Research, University Muenster, Muenster, Germany aff005;  Pediatrics III Oncology, University Hospital of Essen, Essen, Germany aff006;  Department of Paediatrics and Adolescent Medicine Division of Paediatric Haematology and Oncology, University Hospital of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland aff007;  Department of Haematology Oncology, University Children´s Hospital Rostock, Rostock, Germany aff008;  Department of Neuroradiology, HIT 2000 National Reference Center, University Medical Center Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany aff009;  Department of Paediatric Neurosurgery, University of Wuerzburg, University Medical Center Wuerzburg, Wuerzburg, Germany aff010;  Department of Radiotherapy, University of Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany aff011;  Department of Paediatric Oncology, University of Bonn, Bonn, Germany aff012
Published in the journal: PLoS ONE 15(1)
Category: Research Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227693

Summary

Young children with brain tumours are at high risk of developing treatment-related sequelae. We aimed to assess neuropsychological outcomes 5 years after treatment. This cross-sectional study included children under 4 years of age with medulloblastoma (MB) or ependymoma (EP) enrolled in the German brain tumour trials HIT2000 and HIT-REZ2005. Testing was performed using the validated Wuerzburg Intelligence Diagnostics (WUEP-D), which includes Kaufman-Assessment-Battery, Coloured Progressive Matrices, Visual-Motor Integration, finger tapping “Speed”, and the Continuous Performance Test. Of 104 patients in 47 centres, 72 were eligible for analyses. We assessed whether IQ was impacted by disease extent, disease location, patient age, gender, age at surgery, and treatment (chemotherapy with our without craniospinal irradiation [CSI] or local radiotherapy [LRT]). Median age at surgery was 2.3 years. Testing was performed at a median of 4.9 years after surgery. Patients with infratentorial EPs (treated with LRT) scored highest in fluid intelligence (CPM 100.9±16.9, mean±SD); second best scores were achieved by patients with MB without metastasis treated with chemotherapy alone (CPM 93.9±13.2), followed by patients with supratentorial EPs treated with LRT. In contrast, lowest scores were achieved by patients that received chemotherapy and CSI, which included children with metastasised MB and those with relapsed MB M0 (CPM 71.7±8.0 and 73.2±21.8, respectively). Fine motor skills were reduced in all groups. Multivariable analysis revealed that type of treatment had an impact on IQ, but essentially not age at surgery, time since surgery or gender. Our results confirm previous reports on the detrimental effects of CSI in a larger cohort of children. Comparable IQ scores in children with MB treated only with chemotherapy and in children with EP suggest that this treatment strategy represents an attractive option for children who have a high chance to avoid application of CSI. Longitudinal follow-up examinations are warranted to assess long-term neuropsychological outcomes.

Keywords:

Cancer treatment – Cognition – Surgical and invasive medical procedures – Neuropsychological testing – Neuropsychology – Cancer chemotherapy – Intelligence tests – Medulloblastoma

Introduction/Background

Therapy for malignant brain tumours aims to improve survival rates and minimise long-term sequelae. In young children, this represents an extremely challenging task, because some brain tumours in this age group have a particularly aggressive biology and the younger brain is more susceptible to therapy-induced damage. Thus, in the German paediatric brain tumour group, children with medulloblastoma (MB) and ependymoma (EP) younger than 4 years of age were stratified into different risk-adapted treatment regimens [13].

Neuropsychological outcome data are important to weigh the benefits of better survival with more intense therapy against the risks of long-term sequelae [4]. Initial neuropsychological follow-up examinations were conducted with a limited number of patients (n = 34) in the HIT-SKK’87/’92 cohorts. Those examinations showed a significant decline in fluid intelligence after craniospinal irradiation (CSI) treatments and a significantly better outcome in children treated only with chemotherapy including intraventricular methotrexate (MTXi.vt.) instead of CSI [2, 48]. However, it is well established that the neurocognitive outcome can also be influenced by other factors, such as the type and location of the tumour [913], the age at the time of treatment [5, 1416], the type and dose of radiotherapy [5, 6, 15, 17, 18], and the type of chemotherapy [1, 19]. Although chemotherapy alone generally causes less severe late cognitive effects than CSI, it may nevertheless have an impact on neurocognitive functions, such as attention, executive functioning, visual processing, and visual-motor functioning. A close correlation has been established between the total IQ score and visual-motor and executive functions [20]. In addition, other factors might impact intellectual development, such as tumour histopathology, hydrocephalus and its management, or postoperative posterior fossa syndrome (PFS) [21]. A recent review described impairments in 456 survivors of childhood posterior fossa tumours and concluded that MB survivors exhibited substantial restrictions in IQ, attention, as well as executive and memory functions [21]. In contrast, von Hoff et al. reported a surprisingly good neuropsychological outcome in EP survivors; only 2 of 23 patients had impaired IQs [3].

At the time of initiation of the HIT-2000 trial, there was a striking lack of standardization in conducting neuropsychological follow-up examinations, based on functional domains and/or a taxonomic framework [22, 23]. Therefore, we developed and extensively validated two test batteries: the “Wuerzburger Psychologische Diagnostik” (WUEPD) and the Wuerzburg short diagnostic (WUEP-KD), first presented at the ISPNO 2000 [24]. Our group has recently published the validity and reliability of these test batteries for use in children with brain tumours [7].

The present cross-sectional study aimed to confirm the detrimental effects of CSI in a larger cohort of MB and EP patients, recruited from the HIT-2000 studies, 5 years after surgery. We also aimed to analyse the impact of therapy-related factors (i.e., local radiotherapy [LRT], CSI, MTXi.vt.) as well as age, gender, or time since surgery on the IQ profiles of patients by comparing outcomes in the prospective HIT2000/HIT-Rez 2005 trials. Here, we report on children treated before the HIT-2000 trial amendment in 2005, and who were examined between 2007 to 2011.

Subjects and methods

Psychological tests

All tests were applied previously for the diagnosis of treatment-related side effects in paediatric patients with brain tumours in German-speaking countries; the tests are listed in Table 1. As a basis, we chose the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) model of intelligence [7, 24, 25]. Our model-oriented tests were performed with the WUEPD [7] full test battery, which requires about 3 h of evaluation with the patient. The WUEPD assessment of intelligence consisted of the WUEP-KD, a theory-oriented diagnostic tool for neuropsychological follow-up, and the Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) [7, 23, 26]. At the time of study initiation (1996–2000), only the K-ABC and the HAWIK-R were available in a german, standardised version of 1991, however, the latter did not meet the CHC-based requirements.

Tab. 1. Neuropsychological tests and abbreviations.
Neuropsychological tests and abbreviations.
All standardised scores (response variables) were normalised to mean 100, SD 15.

Standard tests commonly lack several important features: they do not differentiate between specific neurocognitive aspects of IQ; they do not discriminate between affected brain areas; and they do not consider brain damage. To provide information about the performance of specific brain regions, the applied subtests must at least distinguish between frontal, cranial, and cerebellar parts of the brain. Importantly, the WUEP-KD mental test battery comprised tests that could detect brain damage independent of the motor response abilities of the tested patients.

To evaluate the time needed to achieve a positive motor response, we examined two variables with computerised tests, the cerebellar time-modulated motor-oriented tap-time (or tapping speed, TS) and the fronto-cortical oriented processing speed (CPT-k with the subtests hits false, decision time, and power, -F, -DT-, -PO). Because these tests were measured separately from mental tests, they avoided the bias of impaired motor function, which might have influenced the results [27, 28].

To assess the degree of impairment in behaviour and participation in daily life situations we employed the “Assessment Scales of Involvement in Life Situations” (Fertigkeitenskala Münster Heidelberg; FMH) [29].

General intelligence was measured with the K-ABC, which included the total IQ Mental Processing Composite (K_MPC) and the subtests Simultaneous Processing (K_SIM) and Sequential Processing (K_SEQ, Table 1). Central cognitive ability was dertermined as an overall ‘fluid intelligence’ score from the Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices [30] with the standardization of Bulheller & Häcker in 1218 children. This test takes the fundamental cerebral networks into account [6, 31].

To detect disturbances in the appropriation of environmentally oriented features, we used the developmental test of Visual-Motor Integration (VMI) [32]. According to the theories of Luria [33] and Piaget [34], in younger children, shape detection is the best method for analysing feature detection. K.E. Beery showed that the ability of reproducing a shape is part of a complex visuomotoric integration process according to Piaget, and is therefore a self-generated performance of intelligence of the child [35].

For testing verbal functions and the active vocabulary, we applied the Riddles of the K-ABC (K_RI) test. This test explores comprehension-knowledge (Gc) [23]. Short-term working memory (Gsm) was measured with the subtest “Number Recall” (KABC-NR) [26]. To test motoric functions we used a computerised morse key which is able to measure two capabilities separately: First, the higher order domain of cognitive processing speed was measured with a short version of the Connors Continuous Performance Test (CPT-k) for selective attention [36]. Furthermore, we introduced a combined parameter, decision stability or power (CPT-k_PO), which was calculated as the mean of the false rate CPT-k_F and CPT-k_DT. Second, fine motor dexterity was assessed by measuring finger tapping performance with the parameter Tapping Speed (T-SP) [37]. The retest reliability scores for all applied tests were derived from either published manuals or our own studies [7, 38].

Patients

Between January 2007 and April 2011, 104 patients under 4 years of age underwent neuropsychological follow-up examinations. Eligibility criteria for inclusion into the test programme was a history of surgery for a brain tumour and subsequent treatment, according to the procedures specified in the multi-centre HIT2000 and HIT-REZ2005 trials. The clinical trial was reviewed and approved by the IRB of the University of Würzburg and the competent authorities. All participants or their guardians gave their informed consent. Although some patients were lost to follow-up, the vast majority of patients in the two trials were stringently tested in a standardised way; thus, the results can be considered representative of the entire HIT2000/HIT-REZ2005 study population. The patients underwent a neuropsychological examination at a median of 4.9 years after first surgery [1, 2]. To minimise fluctuations due to inter-rater reliability, all examinations were carried out in the respective centres on site by one trained neuropsychologist. Of the 104 tested patients, 23 and 9 patients had to be excluded due to treatment deviations from the protocol or due to divergent histology. Thus, 72 patients with MB and EP were included in the final analysis. Details on patients, the different treatment arms, including the intended and actually applied radiation dosages are detailed in Table 2.

Tab. 2. Treatment groups of medulloblastoma and ependymoma in children < 4 of age.
Treatment groups of medulloblastoma and ependymoma in children < 4 of age.

Treatments

Children with localised MB received systemic chemotherapy and MTXi.vt. to avoid CSI. Until the 2005 amendment, all patients with MB were treated primarily with SKK chemotherapy, and received CSI only when there were metastases or in case of a non-response to chemotherapy. MB patients with metastasis were treated with combinations of induction chemotherapy, response-adapted high-dose chemotherapy and CSI. Children with EP received the HIT-SKK chemotherapy, but without MTXi.vt., followed by local radiotherapy (LRT) [39, 40]. For this neuropsychologcial outcome study, children with MB were categorised into three groups: primary disease without metastases that received polychemotherapy, including MTXi.vt. (MBP, n = 19), relapsed without metastases that received polychemotherapy, including MTXi.vt. and CSI (MBR, n = 5), and primary disease with metastases that received MTXi.vt. and CSI (MBRM, n = 6). Those with EP were categorised as: infratentorial (EPI, n = 32; n = 5 grade II, n = 27 grade III) and supratentorial (EPS, n = 10; all grade III).

Statistical analysis

Individual age-corrected test scores are standard scores (SS), synonymous to IQ. This mean, non-age-related IQ-score is 100 with a single standard deviation (SD) of 15. All applied tests are listed in Table 1.

Univariable distributions of metric variables are described by mean and SD, when data are normally distributed. Otherwise, they are described by median and range. For categorical variables, absolute frequencies are stated. Associations between a normally distributed metric outcome and nominal predictor variables were assessed with the t-test or analysis of variance (ANOVA), depending on whether two or more independent samples were compared.

For multivariable analyses, linear fixed effect models were applied using a stepwise variable selection procedure recommended by Collett [41] to analyse the simultaneous impact of the variables age at surgery (continuous: years), time from start of treatment to neuropsychological testing (TtoNT, continuous: years), gender (binary), and treatment group (nominal: MBP, MBR, MBRM, EPI, EPS). This allows to estimate adjusted mean neuropsychological scores (by treatment group), adjusted for potential heterogeneity in the distribution of age at surgery, gender and TtoNT. Missing values were treated as missing at random (MAR).

Analyses were performed with the SPSS software package (version 24; IBM Inc., Armonk, NY, USA). All analyses were considered as exploratory and p-values were interpreted descriptively.

Results

Univariable analysis

The median age of the 72 patients (48 males, 24 females) included was 2.3 years (range 0.6–3.8 years) at first surgery. The median age at neuropsychological testing was 7.5 years (range 4.5–12.0 years) and the median time from surgery to testing 4.9 (3.6–8.6). Fig 1 visualises results of 10 of the applied test batteries in the five treatment cohorts. Standard deviations were generally high indicating substantial variability in the different scores. In view of the small sample size, the EP subgroups (EPI, EPS) were not further divided into groups of patients with and without relapse.

Fig. 1. Neuropsychological outcome of children with MB and EP 4.9 years after surgery.
Neuropsychological outcome of children with MB and EP 4.9 years after surgery.
Test results of ten different neuropsychological tests in the five different treatment groups are shown. Results are displayed as means ± standard deviation.

For the majority of neuropsychological scores (CPM, VMI, K-NR, K-RI, K-MPC, K-SIM, K-SEQ, T-SP, CPT-k_PO, and FMH) ANOVA suggested that treatment had statistically noticeable impact on scores (Fig 1). EP subgroups generally had higher scores than MB patients. Within the MB subgroups, patients without CSI (MBP) exhibited better performance than patients receiving CSI (MBR and MBRM). Notably, tapping speed and the executive function ‘decision time’ was reduced in all groups without a relevant difference between the groups, irrespective of whether CSI was applied or not (Fig 1).

Multivariable analysis

Multivariable analysis revealed that gender and TtoNT had no relevant impact on any of the neuropsychological outcome scores (data not shown); i.e. these variables were not selected for the final model. As expected from univariable testing, treatment modalities had a highly noticeable impact on all scores from the WUEP-KD, K-ABC, CPT-k PO, and FMH (Table 4). For the scores from the WUEP-KD and the K-ABC, treatment modality was the only selected prognostic factor (Table 3, i.e. the multivariable model reproduces the univariable analysis). In contrast, outcomes in executive capabilites (CPT-k PO) and participation in daily life (FMH) were effected by both age at surgery and treatment modality; i.e. age at surgery and treatment modality both remained as parameters for model calculations of predicted scores (Table 3).

Tab. 3. Results of multivariable model building, summarised over all neuropsychological scores.
Results of multivariable model building, summarised over all neuropsychological scores.
For each neuropsychological score, the multivariable model building assessed the simultaneous prognostic impact of the variables treatment group (nominal: MBP, MBR, MBRM, EPI, EPS) and age at surgery (continuous: years). P-values indicate whether the potential explanatory variable was identified as independent prognostic factor in the multivariable model. Not selected variables were indicated by “N/S”.
Tab. 4. Multivariable linear model for WUEP-KD score at the 5-year follow-up for the complete cohort of patients.
Multivariable linear model for WUEP-KD score at the 5-year follow-up for the complete cohort of patients.
Bold indicates statistically noticeable estimated differences. * Wald test. ** Predicted means adjusted for mean age at surgery.

The predicted mean scores of the various neuropsychological tests calculated in the final model are given in Table 4 along with the estimated differences between the MBP (reference score) group and the other four treatment groups: children with EPI always achieved highest scores followed by children with either EPS or MBP. All patient groups treated with CSI performed substantially worse than children treated without CSI. This pattern was also sustained for participation in daily life situations (FMH questionnaire). These data confirm that the different treatment modalities remain the prime prognostic factors for neuropsychological outcome. Compared to that other factors such as age at surgery are only of subordinate significance for IQ development of childhood brain tumor survivors.

Discussion

In the present study, we assessed the neuropsychological outcome of 72 consecutive children with MB or EP that were treated with protocols specified in the HIT2000 and HIT-REZ-2005 trials. Although this cohort represented one of the largest groups of children analysed with a uniform test battery to date, the numbers of patients included in the subgroups were still relatively small. Multivariable analysis revealed that age at surgery, gender, and TtoNT had essentially no relevant impact on the test scores. In order to minimise the influence of unequal testing intervals we only included children tested 5 years after diagnosis into the analysis.

The multivariable analysis confirmed that both groups treated with CSI (MBR and MBRM) displayed inferior intellectual functions compared to those treated without CSI. Analyses revealed a difference in the predicted means of up to 35 IQ points between the children treated only with chemotherapy including MTXi.vt. and those that received additional CSI. Furthermore, these data show that a deferral of CSI beyond the age of 18 months was not sufficient to prevent cranial radiation-induced injuries to the CNS [42].

The devastating effect of irradiation on the developing brain of young children was initially demonstrated in the HIT’87/’92 studies. Those data also demonstrated that chemotherapy including MTXi.vt. was significantly less harmful than CSI [2, 7]. CPM test results in our study confirmed the detrimental impact of CSI on fluid intelligence. Recent MRI data support these findings by showing areas of reduced cortical thickness after MB treatment (including CSI) and a link between intellectual performance and the right prefrontal white matter volume [31, 43]. As reported before, CSI also significantly reduced working memory (K_NR) in surviving children. In contrast to irradiation, chemotherapy and MTXi.vt. alone appeared to be less toxic. IQ scores in children receiving chemotherapy and MTXi.vt. without CSI (group MBP) remained within normal age-adjusted limits for standard score results [2, 44]. Importantly, the clinical noninferiority of MTXi.vt. as a substitute for CSI has also been demonstrated, at least in young patients with MB that displayed a desmoplastic or extensive nodularity histology, confirming that irradiation is dispensable for children with low-risk MB [1]. As a logical development, the 2005 amendment attempted to prevent CSI for a greater number of patients with metastasised MB, implementing an intensified induction chemotherapy to improve initial tumour control followed by high-dose chemotherapy instead of CSI for consolidation. In summary, our data consistently show that CSI represented the key risk factor for intellectual damage in young children with MB [1, 4447]. Whether upfront MTXi.vt. contributes to CSI toxicitiy remains to be investigated in future trials or subgroup analyses.

Furthermore, it is conceivable that other factors, such as hydrocephalus or posterior fossa sydrome (PFS), might also have impacted intellectual development. Unfortunately, hydrocephalus and its treatment was not recorded as a separate risk factor in the respective trials. In our cohort, we could detect only minor speaking abnormalities, such as low voice, slowed speech, and articulation weaknesses, in 16 patients with postoperative symptoms of PFS 5 years after surgery. However, to date, PFS has been an ill-defined entity; therefore, these data warrant confirmation in trials with a specialised design.

A comparison of the two EP cohorts offered further important insights. Previously, we reported overall IQ scores within the normal range for 23 children with EP treated with LRT [3]. Based on the present study results, we could further differentiate between groups of children with different tumour locations. In fluid intelligence, patients with supra- as well as infratentorial EPs had the same outcome as patients with MB treated without CSI. Similarly, both EP groups showed surprisingly good results on the K-ABC cognitive domain scores and the more central cognitive WUEP-KD scores. Only speed tapping scores were below average in all groups, including both EP groups, indicating that tumour location alone is not a predictor of motor deficits. Deficits in tapping scores are particularly suited to demonstrate detrimental effects of the tumour and its treatment on cerebellar time modulation. In contrast to tapping speed, cognitive motor decisions (CPT_PO) were only delayed in patients receiving CSI (groups MBR and MBRM), showing that motor and psychomotor processing speed is differentially affected by brain tumor treatment.

Although EP tumour location did not seem to play a pivotal role in the neuropsychological outcome in our cohort, IQ scores tended to be lower in the EPS group than in the EPI group, possibly due to the closer anatomic proximity of the tumour to the frontal lobe and cortical regions. The precise anatomic tumour location was not considered an independent variable in our cohort. Perhaps the most relevant new finding from our study is that patients with MB treated with chemotherapy and MTXi.vt. alone displayed equivalent or almost equivalent IQ scores than both EP groups treated with LRT without MTXi.vt. on all tests that were not related to motor functioning (differences were 4.4–13.5 IQ points). This observation might partly be explained by divergent tumour genetics or different anatomic locations. However, it indicates that irradiation limited to the involved tumour field appears to be far less toxic than CSI, and that the HIT-SKK chemotherapy with MTXi.th. is an acceptable option e.g. for young children with non-metastatic desmoplastic MB, both in terms of survival and long term IQ development. Data from the FMH questionnaire yielded results very similar to those from the WUEPD and WUEP-KD IQ batteries. The results illustrate the relevance of IQ for daily life situations. Therefore, the more feasible FMH questionnaire might be equally suited to identifying affected populations, compared to the more time consuming test batteries, albeit in less detail. For example, the FMH questionnaire cannot provide a sophisticated profile that can inform the design of deficit-specific rehabilitation programmes. For these purposes, the more in-depth analysis profile of the aforementioned test batteries would be required. This distinction is particularly important in examining young children, where a wide range of cognitive abilities must be investigated [48], due to the greater prevalence of neuropsychological deficits in the young age group compared to older children. Previous studies in young patients with brain tumours mainly used arbitrarily selected tests, which were not based on a precise model, like the CHC [49]. However, our data clearly showed that it is of prime importance to implement theory-driven neuropsychological test systems in this vulnerable patient population [23]. In this respect, a recently established consensus between 18 participating European countries for an internationally accepted test battery for follow-up of childhood ependymoma survivors, that can also be used for other brain tumors including medulloblastoma, represents a big leap forward. The ‘Core-Plus’ concept aims to establish a minimum dataset where resources are limited, whilst maintaining scope for a more comprehensive assessment where feasible. The model represents a significant improvement in the ability for international collaboration using the same analogous measures, which in the core battery relies upon the Wechsler IQ test to obtain IQ estimate, Verbal, Working Memory and Processing Speed. This core battery is combined with a reading subtest, a pegboard test to assess fine motor functioning, tapping speed and the CPT, the Beery VMI and Ravens Matrices, all tests that were already used in this current study. Furthermore, the additional tests include gold standard measures of executive functioning, memory, attention and academics, which are extremely important domains to clearly elucidate the late effects of paediatric brain tumor treatment, that will also drive the focus of future interventions.

Conclusions

The WUEPD/WUEP-KD tests proved to be a reliable tool for measuring the cognitive outcome of paediatric patients with brain tumours, and were the starting point of the recently proposed CorePlus consensus concept of the European Paediatric Brain Tumour Group for assessment of survivors of childhood brain tumours over five years of age [7, 49].

Our data identify type of treatment as the most relevant independent risk factor for neuropsychological outcomes in young children under 4. Children receiving CSI and tested 4.9 years after surgery displayed severe limitations in key areas of cognitive development, motor function and selective attention. With respect to motoric functions, all patients displayed significant deficits in motor speed, however, reduction of cognitive motor decision time was only prevalent in children receiving CSI. Importantly, IQ scores of MB children treated with MBP (including MTXi.vt.) reached almost equivalent to IQ-scores of children with EP, suggesting that MBP treatment is an interesting option for children with low-risk MB who have a high chance to avoid the application of CSI. Due to the multitude of possible influencing factors, the limited number of patients in our cohort, and the clinical trial design, we could not consider all relevant parameters. Future studies are needed to determine the impact of the exact anatomic tumour location, of intrathecal chemotherapy on subsequent radiotherapy, and surgical complications on neuropsychological outcome. Longitudinal studies that focus more on IQ development over time are currently under way.


Zdroje

1. Rutkowski S, Gerber NU, von Hoff K, Gnekow A, Bode U, Graf N et al: Treatment of early childhood medulloblastoma by postoperative Chemotherapy and deferred radiotherapy. Neuro-Oncology 2009, 11(2):201–210. doi: 10.1215/15228517-2008-084 18818397

2. Rutkowski S, Bode U, Deinlein F, Ottensmeier H, Warmuth-Metz M, Soerensen N et al: Treatment of early childhood medulloblastoma by postoperative chemotherapy alone. The New England journal of medicine 2005, 352(10):978–986. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa042176 15758008

3. von Hoff K, Kieffer V, Habrand JL, Kalifa C, Dellatolas G, Grill J: Impairment of intellectual functions after surgery and posterior fossa irradiation in children with ependymoma is related to age and neurologic complications. BMC Cancer 2008, 8:15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2407-8-15 18208613

4. Palmer SL: Neurodevelopmental impact on children treated for medulloblastoma: a review and proposed conceptual model. Developmental disabilities research reviews 2008, 14(3):203–210. doi: 10.1002/ddrr.32 18924159

5. Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Merchant TE, Wallace D, Kocak M, Brouwers et al: Neurocognitive consequences of risk-adapted therapy for childhood medulloblastoma. J Clin Oncol 2005, 23(24):5511–5519. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2005.00.703 16110011

6. Prabhakaran V, Smith JAL, Desmond JE, Glover GH: Neural Substrates of Fluid Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Neocortical Activation during Performance of the Raven´s Progressive Matrices Test. Cognitive Psychology 1997, 33:43–63. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1997.0659 9212721

7. Ottensmeier H, Zimolong B, Wolff JE, Ehrich J, Galley N, Rutkowski S et al: Neuropsychological short assessment of disease- and treatment-related intelligence deficits in children with brain tumours. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2015, 19(3):298–307. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2014.12.019 25617910

8. Rutkowski S, Gerber NU, von Hoff K, Gnekow A, Bode U, Graf N et al: Treatment of early childhood medulloblastoma by postoperative chemotherapy and deferred radiotherapy. Neuro Oncol 2009, 11(2):201–210. doi: 10.1215/15228517-2008-084 18818397

9. Conklin HM, Li C, Xiong X, Ogg RJ, Merchant TE: Predicting change in academic abilities after conformal radiation therapy for localized ependymoma. J Clin Oncol 2008, 26(24):3965–3970. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2007.15.9970 18711186

10. Palmer SL, Reddick WE, Glass JO, Ogg R, Patay Z, Wallace D et al: Region et al al white matter anisotropy and reading ability in patients treated for pediatric embryonal tumors. Brain Imaging Behav 2010, 4(2):132–140. doi: 10.1007/s11682-010-9092-1 20502994

11. Schreiber JE, Gurney JG, Palmer SL, Bass JK, Wang M, Chen S et al: Examination of risk factors for intellectual and academic outcomes following treatment for pediatric medulloblastoma. Neuro Oncol, 16(8):1129–1136. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/nou006 24497405

12. Palmer SL, Hassall T, Evankovich K, Mabbott DJ, Bonner M, Deluca C et al: Neurocognitive outcome 12 months following cerebellar mutism syndrome in pediatric patients with medulloblastoma. Neuro Oncol, 12(12):1311–1317. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/noq094 20713408

13. Korah MP, Esiashvili N, Mazewski CM, Hudgins RJ, Tighiouart M, Janss AJ et al: Incidence, risks, and sequelae of posterior fossa syndrome in pediatric medulloblastoma. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys, 77(1):106–112. doi: 10.1016/j.ijrobp.2009.04.058 19695790

14. Mulhern RK, Palmer SL, Reddick W, E., Glass JO, Kun LE, Taylor J et al: Risk of young age for selected neurocognitive deficits in medulloblastoma are associated with white matter loss Journal of Clinical Oncology 2001, 19(2):472–479.

15. Mulhern RK, Hancock J, Fairclough D, Kun L: Neuropsychological status of children treated for brain tumors: a critical review and integrative analysis. Med Pediatr Oncol 1992, 20(3):181–191. doi: 10.1002/mpo.2950200302 1574027

16. Nagel BJ, Delis DC, Palmer SL, Reeves C, Gajjar A, Mulhern RK: Early patterns of verbal memory impairment in children treated for medulloblastoma. Neuropsychology 2006, 20(1):105–112. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.20.1.105 16460226

17. Kiehna EN, Mulhern RK, Li C, Xiong X, Merchant TE: Changes in attentional performance of children and young adults with localized primary brain tumors after conformal radiation therapy. J Clin Oncol 2006, 24(33):5283–5290. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2005.03.8547 17114662

18. Kieffer-Renaux V, Bulteau C, Grill J, Kalifa C, Viguier D, Jambaque I: Patterns of neuropsychological deficits in children with medulloblastoma according to craniospatial irradiation doses. Dev Med Child Neurol 2000, 42(11):741–745. doi: 10.1017/s0012162200001377 11104345

19. Riva D, Giorgi C, Nichelli F, Bulgheroni S, Massimino M, Cefalo G et al: Intrathecal methotrexate affects cognitive function in children with medulloblastoma. Neurology 2002, 59(1):48–53. doi: 10.1212/wnl.59.1.48 12105306

20. Pangelinan MM, Zhang G, VanMeter JW, Clark JE, Hatfield BD, Haufler AJ: Beyond age and gender: relationships between cortical and subcortical brain volume and cognitive-motor abilities in school-age children. Neuroimage 2011, 54(4):3093–3100. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.11.021 21078402

21. Hanzlik E, Woodrome SE, Abdel-Baki M, Geller TJ, Elbabaa SK: A systematic review of neuropsychological outcomes following posterior fossa tumor surgery in children. Child's nervous system: ChNS: official journal of the International Society for Pediatric Neurosurgery 2015, 31(10):1869–1875.

22. Robinson KE, Kuttesch JF, Champion JE, Andreotti CF, Hipp DW, Bettis A et al: A Quantitative Meta-Analysis of Neurocognitive Sequelae in Survivors of Pediatric. Pediatric Blood & Cancer 2010, 55:525–531.

23. Schneider WJ, McGrew KS: The Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. In: Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (3rd ed) edn. Edited by Flanagan DP. New York, NY: US: Guilford Press; 2012: 99–144

24. Ottensmeier H, Kuehl J: Pilot Trial HIT-SKK´87/HIT-SKK´92: A retrospective neuropsychological study in children < 3 years of age with medulloblastoma. Neuro Oncol 2000, 2 (Supplement 2):71–96.

25. McGrew KS: CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: Standing on the shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence 2009, 37:1–10.

26. Melchers P, Preuss U: Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) dt. Version., 8 th ed edn. Frankfurt/Main: Pearson Assessment; 2009.

27. Fry AF, Hale S: Relationship among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence. Biological Psychology 2000, 54:1–34. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0511(00)00051-x 11035218

28. Koustenis E, Hernaiz Driever P, de Sonneville L, Rueckriegel SM: Executive function deficits in pediatric cerebellar tumor survivors. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2016, 20(1):25–37. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2015.11.001 26631949

29. Wolff JE, Daumling E, Dirksen A, Dabrock A, Hartmann M, Jurgens H: [Munster Heidelberg Abilities Scale—a measuring instrument for global comparison of illness sequelae]. Klinische Padiatrie 1996, 208(5):294–298. doi: 10.1055/s-2008-1046486 8992097

30. Raven JC, Bulheller S, Häcker H: Coloured Progressive Matrices (3. neu normierte Auflage). Frankfurt: Swets Test Sevices; 2002.

31. Ullen F, Forsman L, Blom O, Karabanov A, Madison G: Intelligence and variability in a simple timing task share neural substrates in the prefrontal white matter. The Journal of neuroscience: the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 2008, 28(16):4238–4243.

32. Beery KE: The Beery-Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration VMI. Manual (5th ed.). Minneapolis: Pearson; 2006.

33. Luria AR: The Working Brain. In., edn. London; 1973.

34. Piaget J, Inhelder B: Mental imagery in the child. A study of the development of imaginal representations. London: Routledge & Kegal Paul; 1971.

35. Beery KE, Visual-motor integration. Monograph. Chicago: Follett Publishing Company; 1967.

36. Rosvold HE, Mirsky AF, Sarason I, Bransone ED, Beck LH: A Continuous performance test of brain damage. Journal of Consulting Psychology 1956, 20(5):343–350. doi: 10.1037/h0043220 13367264

37. Tapping: Computersoftware for the measurement of handedness. In. Wedemark (Germany): Meditech; 2008.

38. Ottensmeier H, Galley N, Rutkowski S, Kühl J: Kurzgefasste Intelligenzdiagnostik bei Hirntumoren. Kindheit & Entwicklung 2006, 15:100–106.

39. Grill J, Le Deley MC, Gambarelli D, Raquin MA, Couanet D, Pierre-Kahn A et al: Postoperative chemotherapy without irradiation for ependymoma in children under 5 years of age: a multicenter trial of the French Society of Pediatric Oncology. J Clin Oncol 2001, 19(5):1288–1296. doi: 10.1200/JCO.2001.19.5.1288 11230470

40. Kuhl J: Modern treatment strategies in medulloblastoma. Child's nervous system: ChNS: official journal of the International Society for Pediatric Neurosurgery 1998, 14(1–2):2–5.

41. Collett D: Modelling survival data in medical research, 1st edn. London; New York: Chapman & Hall; 1994.

42. Muller K, Mynarek M, Zwiener I, Siegler N, Zimmermann M, Christiansen H et al: Postponed is not canceled: role of craniospinal radiation therapy in the management of recurrent infant medulloblastoma—an experience from the HIT-REZ 1997 & 2005 studies. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys 2014, 88(5):1019–1024. doi: 10.1016/j.ijrobp.2014.01.013 24661654

43. Liu AK, Marcus KJ, Fischl B, Grant PE, Poussaint TY, Rivkin MJ et al: Changes in cerebral cortex of children treated for medulloblastoma. Int J Radiat Oncol Biol Phys 2007, 68(4):992–998. doi: 10.1016/j.ijrobp.2007.01.034 17379433

44. von Bueren AO, von Hoff K, Pietsch T, Gerber NU, Warmuth-Metz M, Deinlein F et al: Treatment of young children with localized medulloblastoma by chemotherapy alone: results of the prospective, multicenter trial HIT 2000 confirming the prognostic impact of histology. Neuro Oncol 2011, 13(6):669–679. doi: 10.1093/neuonc/nor025 21636711

45. Dhall G: Medulloblastoma. Journal of child neurology 2009, 24(11):1418–1430. doi: 10.1177/0883073809341668 19841429

46. Saury JM, Emanuelson I: Cognitive consequences of the treatment of medulloblastoma among children. Pediatric neurology 2011, 44(1):21–30. doi: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2010.07.004 21147383

47. Ris MD, Walsh K, Wallace D, Armstrong FD, Holmes E, Gajjar A et al: Intellectual and academic outcome following two chemotherapy regimens and radiotherapy for average-risk medulloblastoma: COG A9961. Pediatr Blood Cancer 2013, 60(8):1350–1357. doi: 10.1002/pbc.24496 23444345

48. Anderson FS, Kunin-Batson AS: Neurocognitive late effects of chemotherapy in children: the past 10 years of research on brain structure and function. Pediatr Blood Cancer 2009, 52(2):159–164. doi: 10.1002/pbc.21700 18680151

49. Limond JA, Bull KS, Calaminus G, Kennedy CR, Spoudeas HA, Brain Tumour Quality of Survival Group ISoPO et al: Quality of survival assessment in European childhood brain tumour trials, for children aged 5 years and over. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2015, 19(2):202–210. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2014.12.003 25617909


Článek vyšel v časopise

PLOS One


2020 Číslo 1
Nejčtenější tento týden
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
Kurzy

Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova

plice
INSIGHTS from European Respiratory Congress
nový kurz

Současné pohledy na riziko v parodontologii
Autoři: MUDr. Ladislav Korábek, CSc., MBA

Svět praktické medicíny 3/2024 (znalostní test z časopisu)

Kardiologické projevy hypereozinofilií
Autoři: prof. MUDr. Petr Němec, Ph.D.

Střevní příprava před kolonoskopií
Autoři: MUDr. Klára Kmochová, Ph.D.

Všechny kurzy
Kurzy Podcasty Doporučená témata Časopisy
Přihlášení
Zapomenuté heslo

Zadejte e-mailovou adresu, se kterou jste vytvářel(a) účet, budou Vám na ni zaslány informace k nastavení nového hesla.

Přihlášení

Nemáte účet?  Registrujte se

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#