Lower IQ and poorer cognitive profiles in treated perinatally HIV-infected children is irrespective of having a background of international adoption
Authors:
M. Van den Hof aff001; A. M. ter Haar aff001; H. J. Scherpbier aff001; P. Reiss aff002; F. W. N. M. Wit aff002; K. J. Oostrom aff005; D. Pajkrt aff001
Authors place of work:
Emma Children’s Hospital, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Pediatric Infectious Diseases, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
aff001; Department of Global Health, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam and Amsterdam Institute for Global Health and Development, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
aff002; HIV Monitoring Foundation, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
aff003; Department of Internal Medicine, Division of Infectious Diseases, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, and Amsterdam Infection and Immunity Institute, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
aff004; Emma Children’s Hospital, Amsterdam University Medical Centers, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam, Psychosocial Department, Amsterdam, the Netherlands
aff005
Published in the journal:
PLoS ONE 14(12)
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0224930
Summary
Background
HIV-associated cognitive deficiency in perinatally HIV-infected (PHIV) children has been studied in Western countries in a population of which an increasing proportion has been internationally adopted. Studies often lack an appropriate internationally adopted HIV-uninfected control group, potentially confounding the relationship between HIV and cognitive functioning. This study aims to further elucidate the association between treated HIV infection and cognitive development by addressing the background of international adoption.
Methods
We cross-sectionally studied the impact of HIV on cognition by comparing PHIV children and HIV- uninfected controls, matched for age-, sex-, ethnicity-, socioeconomic status (SES)- and adoption status. We used a standardized neuropsychological test battery to measure intelligence (IQ), and the cognitive domains of processing speed, working memory, executive function, learning ability and visual-motor function and compared outcomes using lineair regression models, adjusted for IQ. We determined cognitive profiles and cognitive impairment by using multivariate normative comparison (MNC) and explored associations with HIV disease- and treatment-related factors.
Results
We enrolled fourteen PHIV children (mean age 10.45 years [1.73 SD], 93% adopted from sub-Saharan Africa at a median age of 3.3 years [IQR 2.1–4.2]) and fifteen HIV- uninfected controls. Groups did not clinically nor statistically differ in age, sex, ethnicity, SES, region of birth, adoption status and age at adoption. PHIV scored consistently lower on all cognitive domains and MNC outcomes. Compared to controls, PHIV children had a significant lower IQ (mean 81 [SD 11] versus mean 97 [SD 15], p = 0.005), and a poorer cognitive profile by MNC (Hotelling’s T2 mean -4.36 [SD 5.6] versus mean 0.16 [SD 4.5], p = 0.021), not associated with HIV disease- and treatment-related factors. Two PHIV (14%) and one control (7%) were classified as cognitively impaired (p = 0.598).
Conclusions
Findings indicate treated HIV-infection to be independently associated with lower IQ and poorer cognitive profiles in PHIV children, irrespective of a background of international adoption.
Keywords:
Learning – Cognitive impairment – HIV diagnosis and management – children – Cognition – Vision – Working memory
Introduction
In industrialized as well as in developing countries, cognitive impairment is increasingly recognized as an important concern in children perinatally infected with the human immunodeficiency virus (PHIV) and treated with combination antiretroviral therapy (cART) [1–3]. The severity of HIV disease and immune suppression, reflected by HIV viral load (VL), Center for Disease Control category C diagnoses and lower CD4+ T-cell counts, have been associated with poorer cognitive functioning in PHIV children [3–6].
In countries such as the Netherlands and the UK, the majority of PHIV children was born abroad, with an increasing proportion having been internationally adopted by adoptive parents [7, 8]. Internationally adopted children are prone to be exposed to childhood adversities, including poor health, economic hardship and compromised rearing environment in their family of origin or within institutions, each of which may have jeopardized their cognitive development [9, 10].
Studying cognition PHIV in industrialized countries, by comparing their cognitive performance with an HIV- uninfected control group of which none have been adopted may potentially introduce biases. Research to date tends to focus on HIV-uninfected (or HIV-exposed but HIV-uninfected) control groups that are matched for age, sex, ethnicity and socioeconomic status (SES), rather than adoption status. Previous results from our NOVICE cohort (neurological, cognitive, and visual performance in perinatally HIV-infected children), indicated poorer cognitive functioning in PHIV compared to controls [3, 11]. However, PHIV children in our cohort were more often adopted compared to the HIV- uninfected control group, this precluding us from investigating any effect of adoption on HIV-associated cognitive impairment.
The current study aims to further elucidate the association between treated HIV-infection and cognitive functioning in children, by addressing the background of international adoption. To study this, we expanded the NOVICE cohort with PHIV children and a well matched internationally adopted HIV- uninfected control group and we cross-sectionally compared cognitive functioning between controls.
Methods
This cross-sectional study was part of the prospective observational NOVICE cohort study investigating the effect of perinatal HIV infection and cART exposure on neurological, cognitive and visual performances, conducted at the Amsterdam University Medical Centers (AUMC), University of Amsterdam, the Netherlands [3, 12–18]. Among all PHIV children in the outpatient department of our hospital we newly recruited those who were 12 years or older between February 2017 and July 2018[3]. We frequency matched HIV- uninfected controls to PHIV regarding age, sex, ethnicity and socioeconomic status (SES), comparable to the NOVICE cohort. We additionally matched for adoption status and, if applicable, region of adoption. We recruited HIV-uninfected children who had been internationally adopted through two government-licensed adoption organizations (Nederlandse Adoptie Stichting and Stichting Kind en Toekomst), since these arrange adoptions from the relevant regions (sub-Saharan Africa). Both organizations used advertisements on their websites and approached to eligible families by email. All participants could speak and understand the Dutch language. We used the following exclusion criteria (as reported by adoptive parents): (non-HIV associated) chronic neurological diseases such as seizure disorders, (history of) intracerebral neoplasms and infections, severe traumatic brain injury (with loss of consciousness longer than 30 minutes), and severe psychiatric disorders. The ethics committee of the Amsterdam University Medical Centers reviewed and approved the study protocol. We obtained written informed consent from all participants older than 12 years and from all parents or legal guardians younger than 18 years if age. This study was registered with the Nederlands Trial Register (ID NL6813).
Sociodemographic, adoption and HIV- and cART-related characteristics
We collected the following sociodemographic data: age, sex, ethnicity, region of birth, substance use (alcohol, tobacco, cannabis and drug use), education level of (adoptive) parents (scored according to the International Standard Classification of Education [ISCED])[19] and number of parents with a job. ISCED is scored from 0 to 9, ranging from less than primary education to doctoral or equivalent level[19]. We defined SES by parental education and parental occupational status. We assessed the following data regarding adoption: adoption status (yes/no), adoption categorized as special need adoption (yes/no) meaning that a child needs extra care and attention (for example HIV diagnosis), and age at adoption which we defined based on the date of entry into the Netherlands. For PHIV children, we performed laboratory testing of HIV-1 RNA viral load (VL) and CD4+ T-cell count on the same day as the neuropsychological assessment (NPA). In all controls we performed HIV-testing to confirm their HIV- uninfected status. The Dutch HIV Monitoring Foundation provided historical data (age at HIV diagnosis, route of HIV transmission, age at cART initiation duration of cART, time between HIV diagnosis and cART initiation) and data since registration in the Netherlands (AIDS-defining clinical events, Centers for Disease Control and Prevention [CDC] clinical staging, nadir CD4+ T-cell z-score, zenith VL). cART was defined as use of at least three antiretroviral drugs from two or more classes.
Neuropsychological assessment
One well-trained neuropsychologist (AMtH) who was blinded with respect to the participants’ HIV status performed the NPA in all children (S1 Table). NPA included subtests of the Dutch version of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC)–III (<16 years of age) or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS)–III (>16 years of age): Vocabulary, Arithmetic, Block Design, Picture Arrangement, Digit Span, Coding and Symbol search[20, 21]. Furthermore, NPA included the Trail Making Test (TMT) part A and part B[22], the Dutch adaptation of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (RAVLT) (immediate [sum of trials 1–5] and delayed recall scores and recognition score)[23], and the Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual-Motor Integration (Beery VMI)[24]. NPA took 1.5–2 hours, including short breaks to prevent fatigue.
Data processing
We standardized WISC or WAIS raw subtests into Wechsler norm scores (with a mean of 10 and standard deviation [SD] of 3) and into scale scores (mean 100, SD 15) using age- and sex-adjusted Dutch norm standards from test manuals[20, 21]. We converted Beery VMI raw scores into t-scores (with a mean of 50 and SD of 10)[24], and standardized TMT and RAVLT raw scores into Z-scores, as no appropriate Dutch reference values were available[22, 23, 25]. We calculated Z-scores based on the mean and SD of the control group [25]. We reverse coded variables in order to align interpretation in the same direction: for all tests lower scores indicate poorer test performance. We summarized subtests into the following aggregated domains: intelligence quotient (IQ) working memory, processing speed, learning abilities, executive functioning and visual-motor functioning, based on a principal component analysis (PCA), as described previously [11]. (S1 Table). We summarized all subtests into these cognitive domains to reduce the number of dependent variables (and, thereby to lower the chance of Type I errors) and to draw conclusions on clinically relevant domains.
Multivariate normative comparison
We used multivariate normative comparison (MNC) to compare complete cognitive profiles to a norm group, by performing one comparison in a multivariate manner [26–28]. This statistical method may be thought of as a multivariate version of Student’s t-test for one sample and can be used to statistically compare a complete cognitive profile to the distribution of cognitive profiles of the HIV-uninfected control sample, taking the covariance between all test scores into account, rather than comparing results of individual tests to the control sample[26]. MNC provides a dichotomous result, which indicates whether each participant is classified as cognitively impaired vs not cognitively impaired, and a continuous outcome, reflecting the degree of cognitive deviation of each participant compared to the control sample (represented by the Hotelling’s T2). To perform MNC analysis, we included scores of IQ and processing speed, and the individual subtest scores of executive functioning, learning and visual-motor function. We excluded the recognition subtest of learning ability due to the skewed distribution. As only complete cases can be analyzed in MNC, we imputed missings in outcome variables using the mean of the group.
Statistical analysis
We compared descriptive data for sociodemographic variables using the unpaired t-test for normally distributed data, the Mann-Whitney U test for non-normally distributed data, and the Fischer’s exact test for categorical data.
To investigate the association between HIV and cognitive performance we used linear regression models with each cognitive domain as continuous outcome. We adjusted the domains of processing speed, working memory, executive function, learning ability and visual-motor function for IQ in order to prevent possible obscuring of IQ effects on outcomes of other domains [11].
Among PHIV group, we explored associations between cognitive outcomes which significantly differed between groups and the following HIV- and treatment-related characteristics: having history of an AIDS defining disease (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, category C), nadir CD4+ T-cell z-score, zenith HIV VL and age at cART initiation.
For data acquisition and management, we used OpenClinica software (version 3.6) and we performed statistical analysis using R (R versions 1.1.383)[29]. We considered a two-sided p < 0.05 as statistically significant.
Results
Participants
We identified nineteen PHIV children from the outpatient department of our hospital, of whom five (28%) declined consent and one (5%) did not meet the inclusion criteria. Thirteen (68%) provided consent to participate. We additionally included one PHIV child, receiving care in another treatment center, whose adoptive parents approached us in response to the advertisements for healthy adopted controls.
Table 1 compares relevant sociodemographic characteristics between PHIV children and HIV- uninfected controls and presents HIV-related characteristics of PHIV children. Participants had a mean age of 10.5 years (SD 1.7) at time of assessment. Thirteen out of the fourteen PHIV children (93%) and twelve out of fifteen HIV- uninfected controls (80%) had a history of international adoption. Groups did not differ, either clinically or statistically, in terms of age, sex, ethnicity, SES of the (adoptive) parents, adoption status, region of birth and age at adoption. The majority of children in both groups (54% in PHIV children and 77% in HIV- uninfected controls) was adopted by a double-income couple with high educational level (85% PHIV and 93% HIV- uninfected controls were living in families with a highest ISCED score of 6–8). Children had been adopted at a mean age of 3.4 years (SD 2.1) and the majority had been born in sub-Saharan Africa. PHIV were diagnosed at a median age of 2.1 years (interquartile range [IQR] 0.3–3.5). All PHIV children were on cART and virologically suppressed at the time of assessment. They had initiated treatment at a median age of 3.07 years old (IQR 1.06–5.41), with a mean duration of 6.8 years (SD 2.6) at the time of assessment.
Cognitive functioning
Table 2 presents the outcomes of NPA in PHIV and HIV- uninfected children for each separate domain. One PHIV participant (7%) had missing data for the TMT-B subtest of the domain executive functioning and one HIV-uninfected participant (7%) had missing data for the recognition subtest of the RAVLT of the executive functioning domain. For these two participants we based the domain scores on the other subtests of the domain.
Compared to HIV-uninfected participants, PHIV children scored significantly poorer on the domain IQ (unadjusted beta coefficient -15.57, 95% confidence interval [CI] -25.93 to -5.21, p-value = 0.005). Moreover, the PHIV group scored lower on working memory (unadjusted beta coefficient: -1.59, 95% CI -3.06 to -0.12, p-value = 0.035) and visual-motor function (unadjusted beta coefficient: -8.35, 95% CI -15.10 to -1.59, p-value = 0.017). Following adjustment for IQ, both differences were no longer statistically significant (working memory: adjusted beta coefficient -0.65, 95% -2.23 to 0.93, p-value = 0.406; and visual-motor function: -3.81, 95% -10.98 to 3.37, p-value = 0.286 respectively). The domains of processing speed, executive functioning and learning ability were not significantly different between groups.
Multivariate normative comparison
Using the MNC to classify cognitive impairment, two PHIV children (14%) and one HIV- uninfected control (7%) were classified as cognitively impaired. The number of children being classified as cognitively impaired did not statistically differ between PHIV children and HIV- uninfected controls (p = 0.598). Using the MNC Hotelling’s T2 as a continuous outcome, PHIV children ranged between -13.03 and 1.426, with a median of -3.75 and a mean of -4.36. The HIV-uninfected controls ranged between -12.10–7.71, with a median of 1.37 and a mean of 0.16 (p = 0.021).
Determinants of IQ and cognitive profile by multivariate normative comparison
Among PHIV children, we explored HIV- and cART related determinants of IQ and cognitive functioning measured by MNC (Table 3). We did not find an association between HIV-disease severity markers, such as CD4+ T-cell, log HIV VL zenith and having an AIDS diagnosis, and IQ and cognitive profile by MNC (p>0.477), nor did we find an association between cART-related factors, such as age at cART initiation and time between diagnosis and cART initiation and intellectual and cognitive profile by MNC (all p>0.380).
Discussion
In this cross-sectional study, we aimed to elucidate whether previous found poorer cognition in PHIV children could have been confounded by having a background of international adoption. To adjust for confounding factors that potentially converge with having a background of international adoption, we compared cognitive performance between PHIV children and a well matched HIV-uninfected control group–being similar in having a background of international adoption. Results of this study suggest significantly lower IQ in PHIV children, but no specific cognitive deficiency in additional domains. As these observations are done in a well-controlled study, we may conclude that the observed lower IQ in our cohort of perinatally infected children with suppressed HIV infection on treatment is independent of factors related to international adoption.
Poorer intellectual performance in PHIV children compared to adoption matched peers is consistent with previous evidence from the NOVICE cohort using non-adopted control group [3, 11]. Both cross-sectional and longitudinal evidence from the NOVICE cohort, showed poorer intellectual functioning in PHIV children compared to both Dutch norm group and controls matched for age, sex, ethnicity and SES [3, 11]. The persistent finding of lower IQ in PHIV children, also when matching for adoption status, further supports the association between treated HIV and IQ.
The findings of the current study differ, however, from a recent German study, reporting on IQ in fourteen PHIV children with a median age of 8.2 years old to be not statistically different from the norm, indicating normal intellectual development [30]. Further, they found IQ to be significantly inversely associated with the initiation of cART within the first year of life, warranting early initiation. A possible explanation for the discrepancy with our results might be due to the lack of children in the current study who had initiated cART within the first year of life, lacking power to detect an association.
Longitudinal observation of the NOVICE cohort showed similar trajectories in overall cognitive performance over time compared to healthy peers, yet suggested executive dysfunctioning in PHIV children once having reached adolescence[11]. However in this cross-sectional design–with regards to other cognitive domains–our results suggest no differences between groups in the domains of processing speed, working memory, executive function, learning ability and visual motor function. Unadjusted models however showed significant differences between groups in the domains working memory and visual motor function. A possible explanation for this might be that the association between HIV-infection and both working memory and visual motor function in fact reflects difference in IQ, or there might have been too little power to detect actual significant associations. With regard to executive function, however, delay can still become apparent during adolescence, as we showed earlier [11].
Comparing complete cognitive profiles between groups using multivariate normative comparison[26], our findings indicate a clinical and significant poorer overall cognitive profile in PHIV children compared to healthy controls. Clinically, we found a higher prevalence of cognitive impairment in PHIV children compared to healthy controls, being not statistically significant. Significant differences in cognitive impairment are consistent with those of previous results from the NOVICE cohort study [11].
We found no significant associations between IQ and HIV- or cART-related factors among the PHIV group. It seems possible that these results are due to a lack of data regarding the severity of HIV infection in the children before entering the Netherlands.
Interestingly, we found that the HIV-uninfected controls included in the current study scored on average 10 IQ points higher (95% CI 0.83 to 18.17) compared to the controls of the previous NOVICE assessment, in whom the prevalence of international adoption background was much lower[3]. Higher IQ in the current HIV-uninfected controls might be attributed to the enriched environment of adoptive families, being mostly well above average socioeconomically. The Flynn effect, describing the worldwide increase in intelligence test scores over time, might also have contributed to this[31].
In industrialized countries, no studies are yet available that acknowledge a background of international adoption as a potential confounder when studying the association between treated HIV-infection and cognition. In the event of international adoption, children have often been exposed to adverse conditions in early life, such as adverse prenatal and/or postnatal rearing conditions, loss of biological parents and residing in an institutional environment during early life. These factors all potentially impact normal cognitive development.
Despite being the first study investigating treated HIV-associated cognitive performance with the inclusion of a well-matched internationally adopted control group, a number of limitations needs to be considered. As for any study of this kind, we cannot rule out the occurrence of selection bias. Possible selection bias might have occurred due to a difference in reasons for relinquishment or abandonment across groups, which is partly reflected by the variation of special need adoption across groups and in the Netherlands [32]. Further, selection bias might have been introduced unintentionally due to selective inclusion of healthy controls through adoption organizations. Although we were unaware of any specific selection bias in the recruitment of controls, we were unable to formally assess this.
The study is limited by the lack of information on early life conditions, such as adverse institutional rearing environment, prematurity, and pregnancy conditions [33]. Therefore, we were unable to analyse these variables. Although we matched groups for (adoptive) parental SES, we were unable to take into consideration genetic factors as a potential confounder in cognitive development. Evidence from longitudinal studies show that IQ of adopted children becomes more similar to the IQ of their birth parents with increasing age, indicating a possible progressive influence of genetic factors as these children age[34].
Since this was a single center study with a limited study sample, this study may have been underpowered to detect smaller differences. Due to the low sample size, we were unable to perform extensive multivariable regression analyses or subgroup analyses. Both significant and non-significant results should be interpreted with caution and results should be replicated by larger (preferably longitudinal) studies[35]. Due to the cross-sectional design of this study we are unable to infer causality. Longitudinal studies including PHIV children and internationally adopted controls would be valuable, establish temporality, which is one of the criteria for determining causal inference.
Taken this all together, the results of this study suggest treated HIV-infection to be independently associated with lower IQ and a poorer cognitive profile in PHIV children, irrespective of a background of international adoption.
Supporting information
S1 Table [docx]
Neuropsychological assessment.
Zdroje
1. Phillips N, Amos T, Kuo C, Hoare J, Ipser J, Thomas KG, et al. HIV-Associated Cognitive Impairment in Perinatally Infected Children: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2016;138(5). Epub 2016/12/13. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-0893 27940772; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5079077.
2. McHenry MS, McAteer CI, Oyungu E, McDonald BC, Bosma CB, Mpofu PB, et al. Neurodevelopment in Young Children Born to HIV-Infected Mothers: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics. 2018;141(2). Epub 2018/01/28. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-2888 29374109; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5810606 conflicts of interest to disclose.
3. Cohen S, Ter Stege JA, Geurtsen GJ, Scherpbier HJ, Kuijpers TW, Reiss P, et al. Poorer cognitive performance in perinatally HIV-infected children versus healthy socioeconomically matched controls. Clin Infect Dis. 2015;60(7):1111–9. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciu1144 25516183.
4. Ruel TD, Boivin MJ, Boal HE, Bangirana P, Charlebois E, Havlir DV, et al. Neurocognitive and motor deficits in HIV-infected Ugandan children with high CD4 cell counts. Clin Infect Dis. 2012;54(7):1001–9. Epub 2012/02/07. doi: 10.1093/cid/cir1037 22308272; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3297647.
5. Nachman S, Chernoff M, Williams P, Hodge J, Heston J, Gadow KD. Human immunodeficiency virus disease severity, psychiatric symptoms, and functional outcomes in perinatally infected youth. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(6):528–35. Epub 2012/02/09. doi: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.1785 22312169; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3407294.
6. Smith R, Chernoff M, Williams PL, Malee KM, Sirois PA, Kammerer B, et al. Impact of HIV severity on cognitive and adaptive functioning during childhood and adolescence. Pediatr Infect Dis J. 2012;31(6):592–8. Epub 2012/05/18. doi: 10.1097/INF.0b013e318253844b 22592486; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3424347.
7. van Sighem A.I. BTS, Wit F.W.N.M., Smit C., Matser A., Reiss P. Monitoring Report 2018. Human Immunodeficiency Virus (HIV) Infection in the Netherlands. Amsterdam: Stichting HIV Monitoring2018.
8. Judd A, Le Prevost M, Melvin D, Arenas-Pinto A, Parrott F, Winston A, et al. Cognitive Function in Young Persons With and Without Perinatal HIV in the AALPHI Cohort in England: Role of Non-HIV-Related Factors. Clin Infect Dis. 2016;63(10):1380–7. Epub 2016/10/30. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciw568 27581764; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5091348.
9. Dobrova-Krol NA, van IMH, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Juffer F. Effects of perinatal HIV infection and early institutional rearing on physical and cognitive development of children in Ukraine. Child development. 2010;81(1):237–51. Epub 2010/03/25. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01392.x 20331665.
10. Gunnar MR, Bruce J, Grotevant HD. International adoption of institutionally reared children: research and policy. Dev Psychopathol. 2000;12(4):677–93. Epub 2001/02/24. doi: 10.1017/s0954579400004077 11202039.
11. Van den Hof M, Ter Haar AM, Scherpbier HJ, van der Lee JH, Reiss P, Wit F, et al. Neurocognitive development in perinatally HIV-infected adolescents on long-term treatment compared to healthy matched controls: a longitudinal study. Clin Infect Dis. 2019. Epub 2019/05/21. doi: 10.1093/cid/ciz386 31106812.
12. Cohen S, Caan MW, Mutsaerts HJ, Scherpbier HJ, Kuijpers TW, Reiss P, et al. Cerebral injury in perinatally HIV-infected children compared to matched healthy controls. Neurology. 2016;86(1):19–27. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000002209 26561287.
13. Cohen S, ter Stege JA, Weijsenfeld AM, van der Plas A, Kuijpers TW, Reiss P, et al. Health-related quality of life in perinatally HIV-infected children in the Netherlands. AIDS Care. 2015;27(10):1279–88. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2015.1050986 26272357.
14. Demirkaya N, Cohen S, Wit FW, Abramoff MD, Schlingemann RO, Kuijpers TW, et al. Retinal Structure and Function in Perinatally HIV-Infected and cART-Treated Children: A Matched Case-Control Study. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(6):3945–54. doi: 10.1167/iovs.15-16855 26087360.
15. Blokhuis C, Demirkaya N, Cohen S, Wit FW, Scherpbier HJ, Reiss P, et al. The Eye as a Window to the Brain: Neuroretinal Thickness Is Associated With Microstructural White Matter Injury in HIV-Infected Children. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2016;57(8):3864–71. Epub 2016/07/23. doi: 10.1167/iovs.16-19716 27447087.
16. Blokhuis C, Mutsaerts HJ, Cohen S, Scherpbier HJ, Caan MW, Majoie CB, et al. Higher subcortical and white matter cerebral blood flow in perinatally HIV-infected children. Medicine (Baltimore). 2017;96(7):e5891. Epub 2017/02/17. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000005891 28207506; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5319495.
17. Van Dalen YW, Blokhuis C, Cohen S, Ter Stege JA, Teunissen CE, Kuhle J, et al. Neurometabolite Alterations Associated With Cognitive Performance in Perinatally HIV-Infected Children. Medicine (Baltimore). 2016;95(12):e3093. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000003093 27015179.
18. Van den Hof M, Blokhuis C, Cohen S, Scherpbier HJ, Wit FWNM, Pistorius MCM, et al. CNS penetration of ART in HIV-infected children. Journal of Antimicrobial Chemotherapy. 2017:dkx396–dkx. doi: 10.1093/jac/dkx396 29126299
19. UNESCO. International Standard Classification of Education ISCED 2011. Montreal: UNESCO Institute for Statistics. 2012.
20. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale manual, 3rd Edition. Amsterdam, Pearson Assessment. 2002.
21. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale manual, 3rd Edition. Amsterdam: Pearson Assessment. 2000
22. Reitan R. Trail-making test. Arizona, Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory. 1979.
23. Saan RJD B.G. De 15-woordentests A en B. Een voorlopige handleiding (intern rapport) Groningen, AZG, afd Neuropsychologie. 1986
24. Beery K. E., B. N. A., A. BN. The Beery–Buktenica Developmental Test of Visual–Motor Integration: Administration, scoring, and teaching manual (5th ed.). Amsterdam: Pearson Assessment. 2004.
25. Phillips NJ, Hoare J, Stein DJ, Myer L, Zar HJ, Thomas KGF. HIV-associated cognitive disorders in perinatally infected children and adolescents: a novel composite cognitive domains score. AIDS Care. 2018;30(sup1):8–16. Epub 2018/04/24. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2018.1466982 29681168.
26. Huizenga HM, Smeding H, Grasman RP, Schmand B. Multivariate normative comparisons. Neuropsychologia. 2007;45(11):2534–42. Epub 2007/04/25. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.03.011 17451757.
27. Su T, Schouten J, Geurtsen GJ, Wit FW, Stolte IG, Prins M, et al. Multivariate normative comparison, a novel method for more reliably detecting cognitive impairment in HIV infection. AIDS. 2015;29(5):547–57. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0000000000000573 25587908.
28. Schouten J, Su T, Wit FW, Kootstra NA, Caan MW, Geurtsen GJ, et al. Determinants of reduced cognitive performance in HIV-1-infected middle-aged men on combination antiretroviral therapy. AIDS. 2016;30(7):1027–38. Epub 2016/01/12. doi: 10.1097/QAD.0000000000001017 26752277.
29. Team RC. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. 2018.
30. Weber V, Radeloff D, Reimers B, Salzmann-Manrique E, Bader P, Schwabe D, et al. Neurocognitive development in HIV-positive children is correlated with plasma viral loads in early childhood. Medicine (Baltimore). 2017;96(23):e6867. Epub 2017/06/08. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000006867 28591025; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5466203.
31. Flynn JR. Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin. 1987;101(2):171–91.
32. Department of Justice and Security tN. Adoptie, trends en analyse: Statistisch overzicht interlandelijke adoptie over de jaren 2013 tot en met 2017. In: Security DoJa, editor. 2018.
33. Xiao PL, Zhou YB, Chen Y, Yang MX, Song XX, Shi Y, et al. Association between maternal HIV infection and low birth weight and prematurity: a meta-analysis of cohort studies. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2015;15:246. Epub 2015/10/10. doi: 10.1186/s12884-015-0684-z 26450602; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4599647.
34. Plomin R. Nature, Nurture, and Cognitive Development from 1 to 16 Years: A Parent-Offspring Adoption Study. Psychological Science. 1997;8(6):442–7.
35. Button KS, Ioannidis JP, Mokrysz C, Nosek BA, Flint J, Robinson ES, et al. Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature reviews Neuroscience. 2013;14(5):365–76. Epub 2013/04/11. doi: 10.1038/nrn3475 23571845.
Článek vyšel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 12
- S diagnostikou Parkinsonovy nemoci může nově pomoci AI nástroj pro hodnocení mrkacího reflexu
- Je libo čepici místo mozkového implantátu?
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
- Pomůže v budoucnu s triáží na pohotovostech umělá inteligence?
- AI může chirurgům poskytnout cenná data i zpětnou vazbu v reálném čase
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- Methylsulfonylmethane increases osteogenesis and regulates the mineralization of the matrix by transglutaminase 2 in SHED cells
- Oregano powder reduces Streptococcus and increases SCFA concentration in a mixed bacterial culture assay
- The characteristic of patulous eustachian tube patients diagnosed by the JOS diagnostic criteria
- Parametric CAD modeling for open source scientific hardware: Comparing OpenSCAD and FreeCAD Python scripts