Social information use in adolescents: The impact of adults, peers and household composition
Autoři:
Lucas Molleman aff001; Patricia Kanngiesser aff004; Wouter van den Bos aff001
Působiště autorů:
Amsterdam Brain and Cognition, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
aff001; Department of Developmental Psychology, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
aff002; Center for Adaptive Rationality, Max Planck Institute for Human Development Berlin, Germany
aff003; Faculty of Education and Psychology, Freie Universität Berlin, Germany
aff004; Faculty of Education, Leipzig University, Germany
aff005
Vyšlo v časopise:
PLoS ONE 14(11)
Kategorie:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0225498
Souhrn
Social learning strategies are key for making adaptive decisions, but their ontogeny remains poorly understood. We investigate how social information use depends on its source (adults vs. peer), and how it is shaped by household composition (extended vs. nuclear), a factor known to modulate social development. Using a simple estimation task, we show that social information strongly impacts the behaviour of adolescents aged 11 to 15 years (N = 256), especially when its source is an adult. However, social information use does not depend on household composition: the relative impact of adults and peers was similar in adolescents from both household types. Furthermore, adolescents were found to directly copy others’ estimates surprisingly frequently. This study provides novel insights into adolescents’ social information use and contributes to understanding the ontogeny of social learning strategies.
Klíčová slova:
Adolescents – Behavior – Decision making – Human families – Human learning – Learning – Schools – Social influence
Zdroje
1. Tomasello M. 2009 The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press.
2. Hoppitt W, Laland KN. 2013 Social Learning: An Introduction to Mechanisms, Methods, and Models. Princeton University Press.
3. Kendal RL, Boogert NJ, Rendell L, Laland KN, Webster M, Jones PL. 2018 Social learning strategies: Bridge-building between fields. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22, 651–665. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2018.04.003 29759889
4. Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J. 2011 The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108, 10918–10925.
5. Dean LG, Kendal RL, Schapiro SJ, Thierry B, Laland KN. 2012 Identification of the social and cognitive processes underlying human cumulative culture. Science 335, 1114–1118. doi: 10.1126/science.1213969 22383851
6. Tennie C, Call J, Tomasello M. 2009 Ratcheting up the ratchet: on the evolution of cumulative culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 364, 2405–2415.
7. Henrich J. 2015 The Secret of Our Success: How Culture Is Driving Human Evolution, Domesticating Our Species, and Making us Smarter. Princeton University Press.
8. Heyes C. 2017 When does social learning become cultural learning? Developmental Science 20, e12350. doi: 10.1111/desc.12350 26547886
9. Laland KN. 2004 Social learning strategies. Animal Learning & Behavior 32, 4–14. doi: 10.3758/BF03196002 15161136
10. Rendell L et al. 2010 Why copy others? Insights from the social learning strategies tournament. Science 328, 208–213. doi: 10.1126/science.1184719 20378813
11. Heyes C. 2016 Who knows? Metacognitive social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20, 204–213. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.12.007 26778808
12. Henrich J, McElreath R. 2003 The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 12, 123–135.
13. Rendell L, Fogarty L, Hoppitt WJE, Morgan TJH, Webster MM, Laland KN. 2011 Cognitive culture: theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15, 68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.002 21215677
14. Wood LA, Kendal RL, Flynn EG. 2013 Whom do children copy? Model-based biases in social learning. Developmental Review 33, 341–356. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2013.08.002
15. Buritica JMR, Eppinger B, Schuck NW, Heekeren HR, Li S-C. 2016 Electrophysiological correlates of observational learning in children. Developmental Science 19, 699–709. doi: 10.1111/desc.12317 26074422
16. Crivello C, Phillips S, Poulin‐Dubois D. 2018 Selective social learning in infancy: looking for mechanisms. Developmental Science 21, e12592. doi: 10.1111/desc.12592 28856760
17. Harris PL, Koenig MA, Corriveau KH, Jaswal VK. 2018 Cognitive foundations of learning from testimony. Annual Review of Psychology 69, 251–273. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011710 28793811
18. Legare CH. 2017 Cumulative cultural learning: Development and diversity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, 7877–7883. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620743114 28739945
19. Legare CH, Nielsen M. 2015 Imitation and innovation: The dual engines of cultural learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 19, 688–699. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.005 26440121
20. Morgan TJH, Laland KN, Harris PL. 2015 The development of adaptive conformity in young children: effects of uncertainty and consensus. Developmental Science 18, 511–524. doi: 10.1111/desc.12231 25283881
21. Choudhury S, Blakemore S-J, Charman T. 2006 Social cognitive development during adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 1, 165–174. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsl024 18985103
22. Blakemore S-J. 2008 The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9, 267. doi: 10.1038/nrn2353 18354399
23. Crone EA, Dahl RE. 2012 Understanding adolescence as a period of social–affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 13, 636. doi: 10.1038/nrn3313 22903221
24. Albert D, Chein J, Steinberg L. 2013 The teenage brain: Peer influences on adolescent decision making. Current Directions in Psychological Science 22, 114–120. doi: 10.1177/0963721412471347 25544805
25. Carpenter M, Nagell K, Tomasello M, Butterworth G, Moore C. 1998 Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 63, i. doi: 10.2307/1166214
26. Gergely G, Bekkering H, Király I. 2002 Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature 415, 755–. doi: 10.1038/415755a 11845198
27. Price EE, Wood LA, Whiten A. 2017 Adaptive cultural transmission biases in children and nonhuman primates. Infant Behavior and Development 48, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.11.003 27884395
28. Taylor M, Cartwright BS, Bowden T. 1991 Perspective taking and theory of mind: Do children predict interpretive diversity as a function of differences in observers’ knowledge? Child Development 62, 1334–1351. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1991.tb01609.x 1786719
29. Hannes Rakoczy, Katharina Hamann, Felix Warneken, Michael Tomasello. 2010 Bigger knows better: Young children selectively learn rule games from adults rather than from peers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 28, 785–798. doi: 10.1348/026151009x479178 21121467
30. Zmyj N, Daum MM, Prinz W, Nielsen M, Aschersleben G. 2012 Fourteen-month-olds’ imitation of differently aged models. Infant and Child Development 21, 250–266. doi: 10.1002/icd.750
31. Wood LA, Harrison RA, Lucas AJ, McGuigan N, Burdett ERR, Whiten A. 2016 Model age-based and copy when uncertain biases in children’s social learning of a novel task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 150, 272–284. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.005 27371768
32. Berndt TJ. 1979 Developmental changes in conformity to peers and parents. Developmental Psychology 15, 608–616. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.15.6.608
33. Bar-Tal D, Raviv A, Raviv A, Brosh ME. 1991 Perception of epistemic authority and attribution for its choice as a function of knowledge area and age. European Journal of Social Psychology 21, 477–492. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420210603
34. Blakemore S-J, Mills KL. 2014 Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology 65, 187–207. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202 24016274
35. Rodriguez Buritica JM, Heekeren HR, van den Bos W. 2019 The computational basis of following advice in adolescents. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 180, 39–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2018.11.019 30611112
36. Ruggeri A, Luan S, Keller M, Gummerum M. 2018 The influence of adult and peer role models on children’ and adolescents’ sharing decisions. Child Development 89, 1589–1598. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12916 28777445
37. Lourenco FS, Decker JH, Pedersen GA, Dellarco DV, Casey BJ, Hartley CA. 2015 Consider the source: adolescents and adults similarly follow older adult advice more than peer advice. PLoS ONE 10, e0128047. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0128047 26030134
38. Efferson C, Lalive R, Richerson PJ, McElreath R, Lubell M. 2008 Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution and Human Behavior 29, 56–64.
39. Molleman L, van den Berg P, Weissing FJ. 2014 Consistent individual differences in human social learning strategies. Nature Communications 5, 3570. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4570 24705692
40. Toelch U, Bruce MJ, Newson L, Richerson PJ, Reader SM. 2014 Individual consistency and flexibility in human social information use. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 281, 20132864. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.2864 24352950
41. Mesoudi A, Chang L, Murray K, Lu HJ. 2015 Higher frequency of social learning in China than in the West shows cultural variation in the dynamics of cultural evolution. Proceedings of the Royal Society B 282, 20142209. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.2209 25392473
42. Jayles B, Kim H, Escobedo R, Cezera S, Blanchet A, Kameda T, Sire C, Theraulaz G. 2017 How social information can improve estimation accuracy in human groups. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 201703695.
43. Molleman L, Gächter S. 2018 Societal background influences social learning in cooperative decision making. Evolution and Human Behavior 39, 547–555. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2018.05.007
44. Mesoudi A, Chang L, Dall SR, Thornton A. 2016 The evolution of individual and cultural variation in social learning. Trends in Ecology & Evolution, 215–225.
45. Glowacki L, Molleman L. 2017 Subsistence styles shape human social learning strategies. Nature Human Behaviour 1, 0098. doi: 10.1038/s41562-017-0098 28553662
46. Van Leeuwen EJC, Cohen E, Collier-Baker E, Rapold CJ, Schäfer M, Schütte S, Haun DBM. 2018 The development of human social learning across seven societies. Nature Communications 9, 2076. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-04468-2 29802252
47. Greenfield PM, Keller H, Fuligni AJ, Maynard A. 2003 Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology 54, 461–490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145221 12415076
48. Hewlett BS, Fouts HN, Boyette AH, Hewlett BL. 2011 Social learning among Congo Basin hunter-gatherers. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B 366, 1168–1178. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0373 21357239
49. Singh JP. 2003 Nuclearisation of household and family in urban India. Sociological Bulletin 52, 53–72. doi: 10.1177/0038022920030103
50. Georgas J et al. 2001 Functional relationships in the nuclear and extended family: A 16-culture study. International Journal of Psychology 36, 289–300. doi: 10.1080/00207590143000045
51. Hamilton HA. 2005 Extended families and adolescent well-being. The Journal of Adolescent Health 36, 260–266. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2004.02.022 15737783
52. Krueger PM, Jutte DP, Franzini L, Elo I, Hayward MD. 2015 Family structure and multiple domains of child well-being in the United States: a cross-sectional study. Population Health Metrics 13, 6. doi: 10.1186/s12963-015-0038-0 25729332
53. Kumar A, Ram F. 2013 Influence of family structure on child health: evidence from India. Journal of Biosocial Science 45, 577–599. doi: 10.1017/S0021932012000764 23217628
54. Seymour S. 1983 Household Structure and Status and Expressions of Affect in India. Ethos 11, 263–277. doi: 10.1525/eth.1983.11.4.02a00050
55. D’Cruz P, Bharat S. 2001 Beyond joint and nuclear: The Indian family revisited. Journal of Comparative Family Studies 32, 167–194.
56. OECD. 2011 Doing Better for Families. Paris: Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development.
57. Ruggles S. 2012 The future of historical family demography. Annual Review of Sociology 38, 423–441. doi: 10.1146/annurev-soc-071811-145533 23946554
58. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs. 2017 Household size and composition around the world. See http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/publications/pdf/popfacts/PopFacts_2017-2.pdf.
59. Allendorf K. 2013 Going nuclear? Family structure and young women’s health in India, 1992–2006. Demography 50, 853–880. doi: 10.1007/s13524-012-0173-1 23208783
60. Chadda RK, Deb KS. 2013 Indian family systems, collectivistic society and psychotherapy. Indian Journal of Psychiatry 55, S299–309. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.105555 23858272
61. Molleman L, Kurvers RHJM, van den Bos W. 2019 Unleashing the BEAST: a brief measure of human social information use. Evolution and Human Behavior doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2019.06.005
62. Yaniv I. 2004 Receiving other people’s advice: Influence and benefit. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 93, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2003.08.002
63. Yaniv I, Milyavsky M. 2007 Using advice from multiple sources to revise and improve judgments. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 103, 104–120.
64. Moussaïd M, Kämmer JE, Analytis PP, Neth H. 2013 Social influence and the collective dynamics of opinion formation. PloS one 8, e78433. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078433 24223805
65. Schultze T, Rakotoarisoa A-F, Schulz-Hard S. 2015 Effects of distance between initial estimates and advice on advice utilization. Judgment and Decision making 10, 144–172.
66. Hütter M, Ache F. 2016 Seeking advice: A sampling approach to advice taking. Judgment & Decision Making 11, 401.
67. Moussaïd M, Herzog SM, Kämmer JE, Hertwig R. 2017 Reach and speed of judgment propagation in the laboratory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 114, 201611998.
68. Giamattei M, Molleman L, Seyed Yahosseini K, Gaechter S. 2019 LIONESS Lab–a free web-based platform for conducting interactive experiments online. Available at SSRN: https://ssrn.com/abstract=3329384 or http://dx.doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3329384.
69. R Core Team. 2015 R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. See http://www.R-project.org/.
70. Bates D, Maechler M, Bolker B. 2012 lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes.
71. Soll JB, Larrick RP. 2009 Strategies for revising judgment: How (and how well) people use others’ opinions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35, 780–805. doi: 10.1037/a0015145 19379049
72. Minson JA, Liberman V, Ross L. 2011 Two to tango: Effects of collaboration and disagreement on dyadic judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37, 1325–1338. doi: 10.1177/0146167211410436 21632960
73. Soll JB, Mannes AE. 2011 Judgmental aggregation strategies depend on whether the self is involved. International Journal of Forecasting 27, 81–102. doi: 10.1016/j.ijforecast.2010.05.003
74. Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. 2010 Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broad-based behavioral science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 33, 111–135.
75. Blake PR, Corbit J, Callaghan TC, Warneken F. 2016 Give as I give: Adult influence on children’s giving in two cultures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 152, 149–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.07.010 27552298
76. Zuilkowski SS, McCoy DC, Serpell R, Matafwali B, Fink G. 2016 Dimensionality and the development of cognitive assessments for children in Sub-Saharan Africa. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 47, 341–354.
77. Hruschka DJ, Munira S, Jesmin K, Hackman J, Tiokhin L. 2018 Learning from failures of protocol in cross-cultural research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 115, 11428–11434.
78. Holding P et al. 2018 Can we measure cognitive constructs consistently within and across cultures? Evidence from a test battery in Bangladesh, Ghana, and Tanzania. Applied Neuropsychology: Child 7, 1–13.
79. Jaswal VK, Neely LA. 2006 Adults don’t always know best: preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science 17, 757–758. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x 16984291
80. Vanderborght M, Jaswal VK. 2009 Who knows best? Preschoolers sometimes prefer child informants over adult informants. Infant and Child Development 18, 61–71. doi: 10.1002/icd.591 20047013
81. McGuigan N, Makinson J, Whiten A. 2011 From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology 102, 1–18. doi: 10.1348/000712610X493115 21241282
82. Whiten A, Allan G, Devlin S, Kseib N, Raw N, McGuigan N. 2016 Social learning in the real-world:‘Over-imitation’occurs in both children and adults unaware of participation in an experiment and independently of social interaction. PLoS One 11, e0159920. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0159920 27466806
83. Carr K, Kendal RL, Flynn EG. 2015 Imitate or innovate? Children’s innovation is influenced by the efficacy of observed behaviour. Cognition 142, 322–332. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.005 26072278
84. Dyer JR, Johansson A, Helbing D, Couzin ID, Krause J. 2008 Leadership, consensus decision making and collective behaviour in humans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 364, 781–789.
85. Mäs M, Flache A, Helbing D. 2010 Individualization as driving force of clustering phenomena in humans. PLoS Computational Biology 6, e1000959. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000959 20975937
86. Lorenz J, Rauhut H, Schweitzer F, Helbing D. 2011 How social influence can undermine the wisdom of crowd effect. PNAS 108, 9020–9025. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1008636108 21576485
87. Tump AN, Wolf M, Krause J, Kurvers RHJM. 2018 Individuals fail to reap the collective benefits of diversity because of over-reliance on personal information. Journal of The Royal Society Interface 15, 20180155. doi: 10.1098/rsif.2018.0155 29769409
88. Talhelm T, Zhang X, Oishi S, Shimin C, Duan D, Lan X, Kitayama S. 2014 Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science 344, 603–608. doi: 10.1126/science.1246850 24812395
89. Uskul AK, Over H. 2014 Responses to social exclusion in cultural context: evidence from farming and herding communities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106, 752–771. doi: 10.1037/a0035810 24749821
90. Morgan TJH, Rendell LE, Ehn M, Hoppitt W, Laland KN. 2012 The evolutionary basis of human social learning. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 279, 653–662. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2011.1172 21795267
91. Cross CP, Brown GR, Morgan TJ, Laland KN. 2017 Sex differences in confidence influence patterns of conformity. British Journal of Psychology 108, 655–667. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12232 27861743
Článek vyšel v časopise
PLOS One
2019 Číslo 11
- Jak a kdy u celiakie začíná reakce na lepek? Možnou odpověď poodkryla čerstvá kanadská studie
- Pomůže v budoucnu s triáží na pohotovostech umělá inteligence?
- Spermie, vajíčka a mozky – „jednohubky“ z výzkumu 2024/38
- Skotská studie upřesnila zdravotní benefity aktivního cestování za prací a studiem
- Metamizol jako analgetikum první volby: kdy, pro koho, jak a proč?
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- A daily diary study on maladaptive daydreaming, mind wandering, and sleep disturbances: Examining within-person and between-persons relations
- A 3’ UTR SNP rs885863, a cis-eQTL for the circadian gene VIPR2 and lincRNA 689, is associated with opioid addiction
- A substitution mutation in a conserved domain of mammalian acetate-dependent acetyl CoA synthetase 2 results in destabilized protein and impaired HIF-2 signaling
- Molecular validation of clinical Pantoea isolates identified by MALDI-TOF
Zvyšte si kvalifikaci online z pohodlí domova
Všechny kurzy