The Contribution of Social Behaviour to the Transmission of Influenza A in a Human Population
For infections such as influenza, there are several aspects to the transmission process, including the properties of the pathogen itself, the host immune system and host behaviour. Although it has been proposed that self-reported social mixing patterns can be used to explain the behavioural component of infection – and mathematical modelling studies based on reported social contacts are used routinely to inform health policy – it is not clear how these contacts contribute to individual- and group-level infection risk. By analysing the relationship between social contacts and infection patterns during the 2009 Hong Kong influenza pandemic, we show that infection risk was strongly influenced by the average reported social mixing behaviour of an individual's age group, rather than by their personal reported contacts. We also demonstrate how social contact surveys can be combined with mathematical models to create useful tools with which to study respiratory infections in humans. This should make it possible to predict how the impact of interventions will vary from one population to the next based on their contacts and, potentially, to explain differences in infection attack rates between groups with different mixing behaviours.
Published in the journal:
. PLoS Pathog 10(6): e32767. doi:10.1371/journal.ppat.1004206
Category:
Research Article
doi:
https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1004206
Summary
For infections such as influenza, there are several aspects to the transmission process, including the properties of the pathogen itself, the host immune system and host behaviour. Although it has been proposed that self-reported social mixing patterns can be used to explain the behavioural component of infection – and mathematical modelling studies based on reported social contacts are used routinely to inform health policy – it is not clear how these contacts contribute to individual- and group-level infection risk. By analysing the relationship between social contacts and infection patterns during the 2009 Hong Kong influenza pandemic, we show that infection risk was strongly influenced by the average reported social mixing behaviour of an individual's age group, rather than by their personal reported contacts. We also demonstrate how social contact surveys can be combined with mathematical models to create useful tools with which to study respiratory infections in humans. This should make it possible to predict how the impact of interventions will vary from one population to the next based on their contacts and, potentially, to explain differences in infection attack rates between groups with different mixing behaviours.
Introduction
For directly transmitted respiratory pathogens such as influenza, an individual's risk of infection depends on several factors. As well as host physiology and the immune system changing naturally with age, previous exposure to related pathogens can affect the outcome of subsequent exposures [1], [2]. In addition, infection risk depends on behaviour that generates potentially infectious contacts [3], [4]. One way to measure such behaviour is through surveys of self-reported social contact patterns [4]–[8].
Mechanistic models incorporating data on self-reported contacts are being used increasingly frequently to examine the effect of social mixing on disease dynamics [8]–[11] and to assess potential control measures [12]–[20]. In these models, populations are structured by age, with the force of infection for a specific age group depending on their reported contacts with other ages [3]. Although there is some statistical evidence from age-aggregated cross-sectional serological data that such models capture infection risk [8], [11], [21], it is not conclusive. Further, it is not known what resolution of age-structured model – both in terms of number and size of age groups – reproduces observed attack rates best.
Here, we report a comparison of alternate hypotheses about how age and self-reported social contacts influence risk of infection. We developed a flexible model framework that could incorporate both age and contact behaviour. The population was divided into increasingly finely resolved age and contact classes, with the transmission rate from one class to another proportional to reported contacts between those classes. Our formulation generalised a number of commonly used transmission models (Figure 1): by varying the number of age groups and contact classes, we could implement a simple mass-action model, an age-structured model [8]–[20], or a model in which individuals were structured only by their number of contacts [7], [22], [23]. Using data from a 2009/10 survey in Hong Kong [24], which included both reported social contacts and biologically confirmed infection status, we first explored how different model structures influenced patterns of infection. Next, we assessed to the extent to which each model captured observed attack rates, and established the structure and nature of social contacts that best explained influenza infection risk. Finally, we used these results to identify the resolution of social mixing that likely shaped influenza A/H1N1p transmission in 2009.
Results
First we explored the theoretical potential of age and social behaviour to affect the infection attack rate in different population subgroups (Figure 2A). Incorporating the Hong Kong contact data, but not yet fitting to serological data, our model framework could produce a number of different patterns for the risk of infection. Figures 2B–G show the predicted risk of infection in different models compared with reported age and number of contacts for each of the 762 individuals sampled in the survey. If transmission was based on reported interactions between age groups, an individual's risk of infection was dependent solely on their age. Thus we obtained vertical bands in Figures 2B and 2E. It is noticeable that when close contacts were used, there was a much higher relative risk in school-aged individuals compared with older groups (Figure 2E). If we assumed transmission was dependent on reported contacts rather than on age, we see the opposite pattern: risk of infection fell into horizontal bands (Figures 2C and 2F). Based on existing literature [7], [8], [10], [11], [18], [22], [23], we might expect that a combination of age and contact structure would capture the observed data best (Figures 2D and 2G).
To assess how contributions from age and social contact behaviour influenced risk of infection, we compared model outputs with observed patterns of infection in each group. First, we used a simulation study to test whether our model could correctly identify the ‘true’ model among a range of candidate models. We simulated data for each of the 762 participants from a model with a specific number of age and contact classes and contact type (see Supplementary Text S1 for details), then compared model performance by considering the difference in Akaike Information Criterion [25] (ΔAIC) for each possible model in our framework. For four different simulation models, our framework correctly identified the structure of the original model each time (Figure S1).
Having tested the robustness of our inference method, we considered infection data from the 2009 pandemic in Hong Kong. Figures 3A–B show the performance of models with different numbers of age groups and contact classes. We found that age-based models, parameterised by the average social behaviour of each age group, gave the most parsimonious explanation of our data. The best performing model according to the difference in Akaike Information Criterion had 10 age groups, with only one contact class in each, and assumed transmission was driven by reported close contacts. In both Figures 3A and 3B, additional contact classes led to worse model performance: the best performing models assumed homogeneous mixing within each age group.
Models incorporating transmission based on close contacts gave a good prediction when there were few contact classes, but a very poor prediction when within-age group contact resolution increased (Figure 3B). Some of the models with multiple contact classes in Figure 3B had classes consisting solely of individuals – some of whom had been infected – that had no reported close contacts. The likelihood of such people seeing infection given the model assumptions was therefore zero. To assess whether our results were sensitive to these assumptions, we considered a framework with an additional small background rate of random contact among all members of the population (see Supplementary Text S1 for details). This extra parameter resulted in a non-zero likelihood for all age all contact classes (Figure S2), but did not change the overall pattern in Figures 3A–B.
The best performing model in Figure 3B underestimated attack rates in the under 18s (Figure S3). This was likely because we had not accounted for differences in susceptibility between older and younger age groups to the influenza A/H1N1p virus [26]. Therefore we also considered a model in which the relative degree of susceptibility of over-18s could vary (details in Text S1), denoted by parameter α. With the addition of α to the basic reproduction number, R0, we were using only two free parameters. Figures 3C–D show the performance of different models when this additional parameter was included. The qualitative pattern remained the same, but there was a significant reduction in the AIC for many of the models. The best-supported model, which had 20 age groups, was not significantly different than the saturated model for 20 age groups (likelihood ratio test, 18 degrees of freedom, p-value = 0.993). Even considering the multiple model comparisons in our study, the similarity between the saturated likelihood and our best fit two-parameter likelihood suggests that this framework effectively captures key aspects of these data.
By examining the difference between observed and predicted values we were able to illustrate the reason for the decrease in AIC with increased contact classes. When the population is divided into 10 age groups, and these groups are sorted by the observed attack rate in each, the output from models using all reported contacts (Figure 4A) and close contacts (Figure 4B) is consistent with real patterns of infection. The addition of a second contact class in each age group, creating a total of 20 risk groups, leads to substantially worse performance, with models failing to capture observed attack rates in most at-risk groups by a substantial margin (Figures 4C–D). However, it is interesting that when all reported contacts are incorporated into a model with two contact classes per age group (Figure 4C), model predictions are closer to the observed data than when close contacts are used (Figure 4D).
Figures 5A–5B shows the performance of the age-only models (i.e. the top row in each grid in Figures 3A–D) as the number of age groups increased in small increments. When all ages were equally susceptible, models using close contacts performed significantly better than their counterparts based on total contacts (Figure 5A). When we allowed relative susceptibility in the over 18s to vary, models incorporating close and total contacts both had similar levels of support (Figure 5B), although the model with transmission based on total contacts required a much lower relative susceptibility in older ages (Figure S4). Estimates for the basic reproduction number, R0, are shown in Table S1. As before, the best performing model included transmission based on close contacts (Tables S2). Overall, the results were robust to choice of age cut-off for relative susceptibility: having reduced susceptibility in the over-10s or over-30s instead of over-18s did not substantially change the overall pattern of the AIC (Figure S5).
We used two types of data in our analysis: the reported social contact data and the paired sera. Although we accounted for the observation error in the sera with the Bernoulli distributed likelihood terms in Equation 2, our framework made the assumption that social contacts in our sample were representative of the population. To test the sensitivity of results in Figure 5B to this assumption, we repeated our analysis using alternative datasets generated using bootstrap samples of the Hong Kong data (details in Text S1). The pattern of improvement in model performance as the number of age groups increased remained consistent when these bootstrap datasets were used (Figure S6).
We also assessed how our mechanistic model, which predicted final epidemic size (Equation 1), compared with a simple statistical framework. We considered a two-parameter logistic regression model that predicted infection risk using reported contacts only. Although the regression model performed well when there are 10 age groups, performance grew significantly worse as the resolution of the model increased (Figure S7). In contrast, the final size model generally continued to perform well once a high enough resolution was reached (Figure 5A–B), suggesting that the well-supported regression model relied on the age group boundaries falling at specific intervals (Figure S5), which happened to occur when there were 10 age groups (Figure S8). There was a fundamental difference in the structure of these two models: only the final-size model accounted for infection from secondary and tertiary contacts.
Figure 5C shows age-specific risk of infection in the overall best performing model, which had 20 age groups and variable susceptibility in the over-18s. The model reproduced the observed drop in risk of infection after childhood, and the small peak that occurs in individuals of parental age. In contrast, the equivalent model with 35 age classes did not reproduce this pattern as well (Figure 5D), and hence had less support.
Although we did not use data on which participants were parents, the rise in observed infection risk correlates well with the age groups that reported having a child in their household (Figure S9). To assess which component of the adults' contacts was driving the rise in infection risk (the within-group contacts to other adults or the between-group contacts to other ages), we assumed that certain age groups had no reported contacts with individuals aged 35–50. We saw little change in model performance if we removed the contacts – and hence contribution to the force of infection – of age groups over 20 (Table S3). However, there was a substantial reduction in performance if we assumed that individuals under 20 reported no contacts in the 35–50 age group, and hence made no contribution to force of infection acting upon the 35–50 group. These results suggest that children, through interactions with their peers and their parents, were responsible for much of the observed infection patterns.
Discussion
Using a flexible model framework in which the population was structured by age and/or self-reported contacts, we compared theoretical predictions with serologically confirmed infection taken from a study of influenza A/H1N1p in Hong Kong [24]. We found strong evidence that an individual's risk of infection was influenced by the average social mixing behaviour of their age group, rather than by their personal reported contacts. Further, we found that finely resolved age classes were required to reproduce the observed distribution of infection. Our results suggest that the post-childhood drop and subsequent parental rise in social mixing are a crucial component of the transmission dynamics of respiratory pathogens like influenza.
There are some limitations to our study. We have only considered contact and serological data from Hong Kong: it would be helpful to test transmission models against observed disease prevalence in other countries. Moreover, we assumed that the one-day contact survey was representative of an individual's behaviour over the period of the epidemic. It would be interesting to see to what extent individual contact patterns vary over time: such changes could be measured in a longitudinal study, and compared with population-level variance in number of contacts.
We also used a single parameter to control the relative susceptibility of individuals who were over a specified age. However, a more detailed parameterisation may be required for other viruses, such as seasonal influenza strains [27]. Finally, although participants provided information on their number of contacts with each age group, we did not know which contact class these reported contacts were in. It was therefore necessary to infer interactions between different contact classes from the original egocentric data (see Supplementary Text S1). The age distribution of contacts of individuals in low, medium and high-contact groups follows a similar pattern (Figure S10), which suggests that this assumption of independence is reasonable, at least when it comes to modelling the age-specific of force of infection between different contact classes. However, factors such as clustering and location may also have an effect on the distribution of contacts: a future challenge would be to develop techniques that could incorporate such information and examine the impact on dynamics.
Social contact data can also be collected using electronic proximity sensors, which automatically record participants' interactions, rather than diary-based questionnaires. Such approaches can provide high-resolution information about the frequency and structure of contacts between participants [28], [29]. However, in a large community, a questionnaire-based approach has the advantage that contacts are recorded regardless of whether they wore sensors or not: it is not necessary to include all potential contacts in the study. Therefore, we suggest that both methods have merit, but that self-reported diary-based methods are perhaps the most useful currently because they can be applied to much larger study populations than device-based methods.
The results we present here build directly on recent statistical analysis of these same data in which it was concluded that, at the individual-level, a participant's self-reported social contacts alone were not a good predictor of their odds of influenza infection [30]. Specifically, an explicit age term was always also required when alternate explanatory variables were compared. These two sets of analyses are not inconsistent. The final size model presented here captures the combined risk of multiple generations of infection in the age-specific mixing matrix. Also, in the empirical study itself, we were not able to ask about behaviour immediately prior to infection. If we were to use a case-control design with confirmed currently infectious individuals as cases, we may find a much better correspondence between self-reported contacts and individual infection.
Of the models we tested, the best performing model included transmission based on close contacts. Previous work also has suggested that reported close contacts are a better proxy for parvovirus [11], varicella [11] and influenza [27] transmission than total contacts. However, it is still not clear which types of contact lead to transmission of influenza and how (or if) these risky contacts are reported in surveys of social contacts. Further, models with a large number of age groups generally perform worse under the AIC than models with 10–25 groups (Figures 5A–B). This might be owing to the sample size we used: at a fine resolution, mixing patterns are informed by only a small group of individuals. Ideally, future studies would test transmission models against observed disease prevalence using larger cohorts.
Our results suggest that infection risk is strongly influenced by the average social mixing behaviour of a person's age group, rather than by their individual reported contacts. This demonstrates that self-reported contacts have useful epidemiological value, as the average behaviour of a population can be used to predict individual infection patterns. Further, we have identified the likely social resolution of influenza transmission during the 2009 Hong Kong pandemic. Although different countries have different social and demographic structure, if the key age transitions – specifically, the post-childhood drop in risky contacts and subsequent parental rise – are fundamentally important to epidemic dynamics, it should be possible to profile different countries' social structure and use these aggregate population data to tailor predictions about infection attack rates. Such information would be relatively straightforward to collect, and could prove valuable in the future when targeting potentially costly interventions during an outbreak.
Materials and Methods
Ethics statement
All study protocols and instruments were approved by the institutional review boards of the University of Hong Kong. Written informed consent was sought from each participant aged 18 or above. Written proxy consent was sought from the parent or guardian of all participants aged 17 or below. In addition, the written assent to participate was asked from participants of aged 7 or above and 17 or below.
Data
We used age and contact data similar to that in the POLYMOD study [6], but taken from a 2009/10 survey of 762 participants in Hong Kong [24]. Participants were recruited by random calling of residential landline numbers for Hong Kong. Data were collected by embedding an interviewer-led social contact questionnaire within a serological survey of influenza. On an assigned day, participants recorded contacts who they touched or had a face-to-face conversation with. The mean number of total contacts reported across all participants was 17.5; the median was 8.0. The frequency distribution of contact had a long tail, and we did not find evidence of a preference for reporting ‘round’ numbers ending in ‘0’ or ‘5’ (Figure S11).
As well as a social contact survey, paired sera were used to identify which of the participants had been infected. This was defined as a four-fold or more rise in titre, as measured by a neutralization assay, between baseline and follow-up visit. The assay tested for neutralizing antibody against influenza A/H1N1p. Such tests have been shown to be more sensitive than hemagglutination-inhibition assays: in a 2010 study, also conducted in Hong Kong, 18 of 19 individuals with virologically confirmed A/H1N1p infection exhibited at least a four-fold rise in neutralization titre [31].
Baseline samples were taken between 4 July 2009 and 19 September 2009. Once clinical surveillance indicated that the peak level of transmission had passed, follow-up samples were obtained between 11 November 2009 and 6 February 2010.
In the Hong Kong serological survey, participants could report contacts as being in one of three age groups: age under 20, 20 to 65, over 65. Relative to population size for each age group, under 20s reported fewer contacts with older groups than older groups reported with under 20s. We therefore adjusted the reported values to ensure reciprocity in contacts between each pair of age groups: if ma,b was the mean number of contacts in group a reported by individuals in group b and Pa was the proportion of the population in age group a, we used a maximum likelihood approach [8] to obtain estimates that satisfied ma,bPb = mb,aPa.
Model
We constructed a flexible model framework with which to compare different mechanistic explanations for infection risk, under the assumption that both age and contact behaviour were known. To construct a model with A age groups and C contact classes, we first sorted participants by age and divided them into A groups, each containing an equal number of people; the final class contained fewer individuals if there was a remainder after division. We then divided each age group into a further C classes, based on reported contacts. The contact classes for each age group were defined by sorting the individual reported number of contacts into ascending order, then dividing the age group into C equal parts. The output from each model was the final epidemic size, defined as the proportion of individuals infected in each age and contact group. As a result, we had only one model parameter to specify: the basic reproduction number, R0. In this section, we outline the general model framework, which could be used with any set of reported social contact data; the technical details of how the 2009 Hong Kong dataset was incorporated into the framework are given in Supplementary Text S1.
We used an SIR model for simulations, with individuals falling into one of three compartments: susceptible, infective or recovered (and hence immune). The force of infection acting on age-contact class (a,i) as a result of infectives in age-contact class (b,j) was proportional to two things: the mean number of contacts made by members of (b,j) with age group a, and the fraction of total contacts made by age group a that were with individuals in class (a,i). We defined mai,bj as the mean number of contacts with individuals in age group a and contact class i by participants in age group b and class j. The transmission rate to group (a,i) from group (b,j) was therefore given by βai,bj = qmai,bj/Pai, where q was a scaling factor dependent on the basic reproduction number and Pai was the proportion of the population in group (a,i) [8]. The final epidemic size in each age-contact class (a,i), φai, could therefore be found by solving the following coupled equation [32],
In our framework, a population could be divided into arbitrarily finely resolved age and contact classes (although the maximum number of possible classes – and hence model resolution – would ultimately be constrained by the total number of participants in the social contact survey). Most modelling studies incorporating age-stratified social contact data used between six and twenty age groups [8]–[20]. In contrast, other studies have explored the effects of the distribution of number of contacts on final epidemic size [7], [22], [23], without using age-structure. Our framework encompassed all of these assumptions: depending on how many age groups and contact classes included, the framework produced a simple mass-action model, an age-structured formulation, or a model dependent only on the degree distribution of contacts (Figure 1). As there were only 762 participants in the Hong Kong study, we limited the maximum possible number of age and/or contact groups in a model to 60, to avoid groups containing too few people (Figure 2A).
Relatively susceptibility in older groups
There was evidence that older age groups had some pre-existing immunity to the 2009 influenza A/H1N1p strain [2], [26]. We included an additional parameter to reflect this immunity: individuals above a certain age had their susceptibility reduced by a factor α, where 0<α≤1. The cut-off could vary: in our analysis we considered a reduction in over-10s, over-18s and over-30s (details in Supplementary Text S1).
Statistical inference
Given a set of parameters, θ, we denoted the model prediction for attack rate in age group a and contact class i by φai(θ). Let Y denote the set of neutralization titres for the study group, and Yk denote the titres for individual k. If individual k was aged a and in contact class i, the likelihood of θ given the data could therefore be calculated with a Bernouilli probability mass function,
We then found the parameter set θ that maximises the log-likelihood across all individuals,
The models were compared using the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) [25]. If a model contains k parameters then AIC = 2k–2l. Here k = 1 in the basic transmission model, and k = 2 in the model with variable susceptibility. Note that with 762 participants and only one or two parameters, it makes negligible difference to our results whether we use AIC or AICc, the criterion corrected for low sample size. For each model, we calculate ΔAIC, the difference between the AIC for that model and the AIC of the model with the lowest AIC. The following approximate rules have been suggested when using this measure [25]: models with ΔAIC≤2 have substantial support compared to the best model; those with 4≤ΔAIC≤7 have much less; those with ΔAIC>10 have practically no support compared with the best model given the data.
Supporting Information
Zdroje
1. McLeanE, PebodyRG, CampbellC, ChamberlandM, HawkinsC, et al. (2010) Pandemic (H1N1) 2009 influenza in the UK: clinical and epidemiological findings from the first few hundred (FF100) cases. Epidemiol Infect 138: 1531–41.
2. MillerE, HoschlerK, HardelidP, StanfordE, AndrewsN, et al. (2010) Incidence of 2009 pandemic influenza A H1N1 infection in England: a cross-sectional serological study. The Lancet 375: 1100–1108.
3. Anderson RM, May RM(1991) Infectious Diseases of Humans, Dynamics and Control. Oxford University Press.
4. EdmundsWJ, O'CallaghanCJ, NokesDJ (1997) Who mixes with whom? A method to determine the contact patterns of adults that may lead to the spread of airborne infections. Proc Biol Sci 264: 949–57.
5. DanonL, HouseTA, ReadJM, KeelingMJ (2012) Social encounter networks: collective properties and disease transmission. J R Soc Interface 9: 2826–2833.
6. MossongJ, HensN, JitM, BeutelsP, AuranenK, et al. (2008) Social contacts and mixing patterns relevant to the spread of infectious diseases. PLoS Med 5: e74.
7. ReadJM, EamesKTD, EdmundsWJ (2008) Dynamic social networks and the implications for the spread of infectious disease. J R Soc Interface 5: 1001–7.
8. WallingaJ, TeunisP, KretzschmarM (2006) Using data on social contacts to estimate age-specific transmission parameters for respiratory-spread infectious agents. American Journal of Epidemiology 164: 936.
9. BirrellPJ, KetsetzisG, GayNJ, CooperBS, PresanisAM, et al. (2011) Bayesian modeling to unmask and predict influenza A/H1N1pdm dynamics in London. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 108: 18238–43.
10. FlascheS, HensN, BoëllePY, MossongJ, et al. (2011) Different transmission patterns in the early stages of the influenza A(H1N1)v pandemic: a comparative analysis of 12 European countries. Epidemics 3: 125–33.
11. MelegaroA, JitM, GayN, ZagheniE, EdmundsWJ (2011) What types of contacts are important for the spread of infections?: Using contact survey data to explore European mixing patterns. Epidemics 3: 143–51.
12. AtkinsKE, ShimE, PitzerVE, GalvaniAP (2012) Impact of rotavirus vaccination on epidemiological dynamics in England and Wales. Vaccine 30: 552–64.
13. BaguelinM, HoekAJV, JitM, FlascheS, WhiteJ, et al. (2010) Vaccination against pandemic influenza A/H1N1v in England: a real-time economic evaluation. Vaccine 28: 2370–84.
14. BaguelinM, FlascheS, CamachoA, DemirisN, MillerE, et al. (2013) Assessing Optimal Target Populations for Influenza Vaccination Programmes: An Evidence Synthesis and Modelling Study. PLOS Medicine 10: e1001527.
15. HouseT, BaguelinM, Van HoekAJ, WhitePJ, SadiqueZ, et al. (2011) Modelling the impact of local reactive school closures on critical care provision during an influenza pandemic. Proc R Soc B 278: 2753–60.
16. KeelingMJ, WhitePJ (2011) Targeting vaccination against novel infections: risk, age and spatial structure for pandemic influenza in Great Britain. J R Soc Interface 8: 661–70.
17. MedlockJ, GalvaniAP (2009) Optimizing influenza vaccine distribution. Science 325: 1705–8.
18. RohaniP, ZhongX, KingA (2010) Contact network structure explains the changing epidemiology of pertussis. Science 330: 982.
19. van HoekAJ, MelegaroA, ZagheniE, EdmundsWJ, GayN (2011) Modelling the impact of a combined varicella and zoster vaccination programme on the epidemiology of varicella zoster virus in England. Vaccine 29: 2411–2420.
20. VynnyckyE, PitmanR, SiddiquiR, GayN, EdmundsWJ (2008) Estimating the impact of childhood influenza vaccination programmes in england and wales. Vaccine 26: 5321–30.
21. GoeyvaertsN, HensN, OgunjimiB, AertsM, ShkedyZ, et al. (2009) Estimating infectious disease parameters from data on social contacts and serological status. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series C (Applied Statistics) 59: 255–277.
22. BansalS, GrenfellBT, MeyersLA (2007) When individual behaviour matters: homogeneous and network models in epidemiology. J R Soc Interface 4: 879–91.
23. MeyersLA, PourbohloulB, NewmanMEJ, SkowronskiDM, BrunhamRC (2005) Network theory and sars: predicting outbreak diversity. J Theor Biol 232: 71–81.
24. RileyS, KwokKO, WuKM, NingDY, CowlingBJ, et al. (2011) Epidemiological characteristics of 2009 (H1N1) pandemic influenza based on paired sera from a longitudinal community cohort study. PLoS Med 8: e1000442.
25. Burnham K, Anderson D (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach. Springer-Verlag, New York, second edition.
26. MakGC, ChoyPWW, LeeWY, WongAH, NgKC, et al. (2010) Sero-immunity and serologic response to pandemic influenza A (H1N1) 2009 virus in Hong Kong. J Med Virol 82: 1809–15.
27. KucharskiAJ, GogJR (2012) The role of social contacts and original antigenic sin in shaping the age pattern of immunity to seasonal influenza. PLOS Computational Biology 8: e1002741.
28. SalathéM, KazandjievaM, LeeJW, LevisP, FeldmanMW, et al. (2010) A highresolution human contact network for infectious disease transmission. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107: 22020–5.
29. StehléJ, VoirinN, BarratA, CattutoC, IsellaL, et al. (2011) High-resolution measurements of face-to-face contact patterns in a primary school. PLoS One 6: e23176.
30. KwokKO, CowlingBJ, WeiVWI, WuKM, et al. (2013) Social contacts and the locations in which they occur as risk factors for influenza infection. In review
31. CowlingBJ, ChanKH, FangVJ, LauLL, SoHC, et al. (2010) Comparative epidemiology of pandemic and seasonal influenza A in households. New England Journal of Medicine 362: 2175–2184.
32. RileyS, WuJT, LeungGM (2007) Optimizing the dose of pre-pandemic influenza vaccines to reduce the infection attack rate. PLoS Med 4: e218.
Štítky
Hygiena a epidemiologie Infekční lékařství LaboratořČlánek vyšel v časopise
PLOS Pathogens
2014 Číslo 6
- Perorální antivirotika jako vysoce efektivní nástroj prevence hospitalizací kvůli COVID-19 − otázky a odpovědi pro praxi
- Stillova choroba: vzácné a závažné systémové onemocnění
- Jak souvisí postcovidový syndrom s poškozením mozku?
- Diagnostika virových hepatitid v kostce – zorientujte se (nejen) v sérologii
- Diagnostický algoritmus při podezření na syndrom periodické horečky
Nejčtenější v tomto čísle
- Fungal Nail Infections (Onychomycosis): A Never-Ending Story?
- Profilin Promotes Recruitment of Ly6C CCR2 Inflammatory Monocytes That Can Confer Resistance to Bacterial Infection
- Cytoplasmic Viral RNA-Dependent RNA Polymerase Disrupts the Intracellular Splicing Machinery by Entering the Nucleus and Interfering with Prp8
- HopW1 from Disrupts the Actin Cytoskeleton to Promote Virulence in Arabidopsis