#PAGE_PARAMS# #ADS_HEAD_SCRIPTS# #MICRODATA#

Sub signum coma –  cur­rent view of chronic disorders of consciousnes­s


Authors: A. Škutchanová 1;  E. Kantorová 2;  E. Kurča 2
Authors‘ workplace: Klinika intenzívnej medicíny a anesteziológie JLF UK a UN Martin 1;  Neurologická klinika JLF UK a UN Martin 2
Published in: Cesk Slov Neurol N 2019; 82(1): 19-24
Category: Review Article
doi: https://doi.org/10.14735/amcsnn201919

Overview

Chronic disorders of consciousness are a modern phenomenon. Recognis­­ing first behavioural signs of contact with the patient‘s environment after coma plays a key role in the patient`s access to extended treatment and rehabilitation, as well as in establish­­ing an ef­fective contact with the environment; it is also considered to be an important prognostic sign. Cur­rent western terms in comparison with traditional definition of apal­lic syndrome or the term coma vigile are more descriptive and precise. In recent years, new methods of as­ses­sment, which enable more exact evaluation of brain disorders were established. However, most of them remain experimental and lack clinical availability, which favours clinical test­­ing us­­ing daily objects. Behavioural response as­ses­sment is the standard to compare results obtained with other methods of as­ses­sment. In our work, we present our view on our perception of a patient with a disorder of consciousness and an overview of cur­rent options in identify­­ing the underly­­ing organic substrate.

Key words:

chronic disorders of consciousness – state of minimal consciousness – vegetative state – wakefulness without answering – WS/UWS – MCS – apallic syndrome – coma vigile – diagnostics – behavioural correlate – examination methods

The authors declare they have no potential conflicts of interest concerning drugs, products, or services used in the study.

The Editorial Board declares that the manu­script met the ICMJE “uniform requirements” for biomedical papers.


Sources

1. Tononi G, Koch C. The neual corelates of consciousnes. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2008; 1124: 239– 261. doi: 10.1196/ an­nals.1440.004.
2. Saras­so S, Boly M. Consciousness and complexity dur­­ing unresponsiveness induced by propofol, xenon, and ketamine. Curr Biol 2015; 25(23): 3099– 3105.
3. Ambler Z. Základy neurologie. Praha: Galén 2011.
4. Schiff ND, Nauvel T, Victor J. Large-scale brain dynamics in disorders of consciousnes­s. Curr Opin Neurobio­l 2014; 25: 7– 14. doi: 10.1016/ j.conb.2013.10.007.
5. Fischgold H, Mathis P, Fischgold H. Obnubilations, comas et stupeurs. Supplement 11. Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology. Paris: Mas­son et Cie 1959.
6. Jen­net B, Plum F. Persistent vegetative state after brain damage. A syndrome in search for a name. Lancet 1972; 1(7753): 734– 737.
7. Kretschmer E. Das apal­lische syndrom. Z ges. Neurol Psychiat 1940; 169: 576-579.
8. Laureys S, Per­rin F, Schankers et al. Residual cognitive function in comatose, vegetative and minimal­ly con­scious states. Curr Opin Neurol 2005; 18(6): 726– 733.
9. Multi-society task on PVS. Medical aspect of the persistent vegetative state (1). N Eng J Med 1994; 330(21): 1499– 1508. doi: 10.1056/ NEJM199405263302107.
10. Giacino J, Ashwal S, Childs N et al. The minimal­ly conscious state: definition and dia­gnostic criteria. Neurology 2002; 58(3): 349– 353.
11. Laureys S, Celesia GG, Cohadon F et al. Unresponsive wakeful­lness syndrome: a new name for the vegetative state or apal­lic syndrome. BMC Med 2010; 8: 68. doi: 10.1186/ 1741-7015-8-68.
12. Gos­series O. Disorders of consciousnes­s: coma, veg­etative and minimal­ly conscious states. In: Cvetkovic D, Cosic I (eds). States of consciousnes­s. The Frontiers Col­lection, Springer: Berlin, Heidelberg 2011: 29– 55.
13. Lippert-Grüner M, Angerová Y, Hralová M et al. Pa­cient ve vegetativním stavu a jeho rehabilitace. Cesk Slov Neurol N 2011; 74/ 107(3): 279– 284.
14. Fins JJ, Schiff ND, Foley KM. Late recovery from the minimal­ly conscious state: ethical and policy implications. Neurology 2007; 68(4): 304– 307. doi: 10.1212/ 01.wnl.0000252376.43779.96.
15. Estraneo A, Moretta P, Cardinale V et al. A multicentre study of intentional behavioural responses mea­sured us­­ing the Coma Recovery Scale –  Revised in patients with minimal­ly conscious state. Clin Rehabil 2015; 29(8): 803– 808. doi: 10.1177/ 0269215514556002.
16. Cruse D, Fattizzo M, Owen AM et al. Why use a mir­ror to as­sess visual pursuit in prolonged disorders of consciousnes­s? Evidence from healthy control participants. BMC Neurol 2017; 17(1): 14. doi: 10.1186/ s12883-017-0798-1.
17. Giacino J, Kalmar K. Coma Recovery State –  revised, administration and scor­­ing design. [online]. Dostupné z URL: http:/ / www.tbims.org/ combi/ crs/ CRS%20Syl­labus.pdf .
18. Leon-Car­rion J, Van Eeckhout P, Dominguez-Morales MDR. Review of subject: the locked-in syndrome: a syndrome look­­ing for a ther­apy. Brain Inj 2002; 16: 555– 569. 
19. Gerstenbrand F. Das traumatische Apal­lische Syndrom. Wien, New York: Springer 1967.
20. Vanhaudenhuyse A, Noirhom­me Q, TshibandaLJ et al. Default network con­nectivity reflects the le­vel of consciousness in non-com­municative brain-damaged patients. Brain 2010; 133(Pt 1): 161– 171. doi: 10.1093/ brain/ awp313.
21. Kirsch M, Guldenmund P, Ali Bahri M et al. Sedation of patients with disorders of consciousness dur­­ing neuroimaging: ef­fects on rest­­ing state functional brain con­nectivity. Anesth Analg 2017; 124(2): 588– 598. doi: 10.1213/ ANE.0000000000001721.
22. Kampfl A, Schmutzhard E, Franz G et al. Prediction of recovery from post-traumatic vegetative state with cerebral magnetic-resonance imaging. Lancet 1998; 351(9118): 1763– 1767. doi: 10.1016/ S0140-6736(97)10301-4.
23. Boly M, Gar­rido MI, Gos­series O et al. Preserved feedforward but impaired top-down proces­ses in the vege­tative state. Science 2011; 332(6031): 858– 862. doi: 10.1126/ science.1202043.
24. DiPer­ri C, Bahri MA, Amico E et al. Neural cor­relates of consciousness in patients who have emerged from a minimal­ly conscious state: a cros­s-sectional multimodal imag­­ing study. Lancet Neurol 2016: 15(8): 830– 842. doi: 10.1016/ S1474-4422(16)00111-3.
25. Thibaut A, Bruno MA, Chatel­le C et al. Metabolic activi­­ty in external and internal awareness networks in severely brain-damaged patients. J Rehabil Med 2012; 44(6): 487– 494. doi: 10.2340/ 16501977-0940.
26. Casali AG, Gos­series O, Rosanova M et al. A theoret­ical­ly based index of consciousness independent of sensory proces­s­­ing and behavior. Sci Transl Med 2013; 5(198): 198ra105. doi: 10.1126/ scitranslmed.3006294.
27. Di Per­ri C, Bastianel­lo S, Bartsch AJ et al. Limbic hypercon­nectivity in the vegetative state. Neurology 2013; 81(16): 1417– 1424. doi: 10.1212/ WNL.0b013e 3182a43b78.
28. Aruab J, Bachmann T, Singer W et al. Distil­l­­ing the neural cor­relates of consciousnes­s. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2012; 36(2): 737– 746. doi: 10.1016/ j.neubio­rev.2011.12.003.
29. Boly M, Faymonvil­le ME, Schnakers C et al. Perception of pain in the minimal­ly conscious state with PET acti­vation: an observational study. Lancet Neurol 2008; 7(11): 1013– 1020. doi: 10.1016/ S1474-4422(08)70219-9.
30. Di HB, Yu SM, Weng XC et al. Cerebral response to patient’s own name in the vegetative and minimal­ly conscious states. Neurology 2007; 68(12): 895– 899. doi: 10.1212/ 01.wnl.0000258544.79024.d0.
31. Doušek F. Hejno bez ptáků. Toito 2012.

Labels
Paediatric neurology Neurosurgery Neurology

Article was published in

Czech and Slovak Neurology and Neurosurgery

Issue 1

2019 Issue 1

Most read in this issue
Topics Journals
Login
Forgotten password

Enter the email address that you registered with. We will send you instructions on how to set a new password.

Login

Don‘t have an account?  Create new account

#ADS_BOTTOM_SCRIPTS#